Article in Sociology · August 000 doi: 10. 1177/S0038038500000304 citations 37 reads 5,200 author
Husin Ali has to leave his house in a hurry to fetch his own family from the hospital. He
Download 151.63 Kb. Pdf ko'rish
|
Ethnic Conflict
- Bu sahifa navigatsiya:
- Situation Malay Chinese Malay Chinese Zoo trip (EPvStatus) 74 41
- Wedding invitation (EPvStatus) 62 16 36 35 Shopping (EPvMoney) 47
- Marriage (EPvPersOblign) 28 14 80 69 Child minder (EPvMoney) 23
- Leave the key with his next-door Chinese neighbour 3. Other
Husin Ali has to leave his house in a hurry to fetch his own family from the hospital. He
has been expecting his sister to come at any moment to assist with his family, but he has waited as long as he could. He wonders whether to leave his front door unlocked or to leave the key with his next-door Chinese neighbour. What will Husin Ali do? Ethnic Conflict 489 Table 1
Percentage of Respondents Who Expected a Predominantly Ethnic Preference in 12 Situations Husin Ali Tang Seng Seng Situation Malay Chinese Malay Chinese Zoo trip (EPvStatus) 74 41 57 66 Child adoption (EPvStatus) 67 34 70 82 Wedding invitation (EPvStatus) 62 16 36 35 Shopping (EPvMoney) 47 16 28 14 Renting house (EPvMoney) 31 10 2 4 Marriage (EPvPersOblign) 28 14 80 69 Child minder (EPvMoney) 23 10 30 17 Support boss (EPvPersOblign) 20 39 32 25 Wedding party (EPvPersOblign) 18 1 1 0 Child’s playmate (EPvPersOblign) 14 3 11 9 Playmate home (EPvPersOblign) 6 1 4 1 House key (EPvMoney) 0 0 2 1 1. Leave the front door unlocked 2. Leave the key with his next-door Chinese neighbour 3. Other The answers showed that ethnic preference counted for more when choosing a playmate for a child on a family trip to the zoo, and probably reached a peak in situations defined as those of political competition between Malays and Chinese- Malaysians. Just as there is a scale of situations, so the findings could have been analysed to compile a scale of attitudes. Some men and women will have tended, in all their responses, to value ethnic association more highly than other respondents. One characteristic of the armed conflicts described in surveys like Brogan’s is the influence of activists who present association with co-ethnics (or ‘ethnic purity’) as a supreme value. They refuse to engage in any ‘trade-off ’ in negotiations, insisting that they are ‘not for sale’. These are the mobilisers. Among those whom they seek to mobilise there are many ‘free-riders’, individuals who profess similar aspirations but would sooner not have to make any sacrifice themselves. As a result there are situations which the mobilisers insist should be defined as requiring ethnic alignment, but which others define in terms of status, money or personal obligation. There can be disagreements over the definition of situations. The Petalingjaya research compared the expectations of male and female subjects and assessed the possible influence of family ties by asking respondents how they thought Husin Ali’s mother would wish him to act in the situations studied. Other persons, either ideological mobilisers or members of a peer group, can impose their definition of a situation upon a person contemplating an exit option, and can sometimes conscript him or her to a movement about which he or she feels ambivalent. The research technique could have been used to measure the priority attached to the fulfilment of religious obligations when balanced against secular inclinations. It could also have been used to compare the strength of a shared inclination to align with co-ethnics with a shared inclination to align with co-nationals or with co- believers. Respondents could have been asked to predict how Husin Ali would choose between alternatives involving Chinese-Malaysians and Indonesian immi- grant workers. A question could have been drafted about the employment at a cheaper rate of someone with an Indonesian-sounding name that could have provided an indication of the significance attributed to differences of nationality. Since there are many cultural and linguistic continuities between Malays and Indonesians the ethnic/national comparison might have been seen as less clear-cut by Malay than ethnic Chinese respondents and the question might have evoked different responses from them. Nevertheless, this sort of technique offers possibi- lities for investigating the multidimensionality of group relations in a more systematic manner. 490 m i c h a e l ba n to n Interaction In calculating the possible benefits of collective action, a major consideration must be the likely response of other groups affected by such action. The most dramatic example of ethnic conflict in Malaysia has been the riot which occurred after Chinese Malaysians in Kuala Lumpur demonstrated their joy over the 1969 election results. Had they been more aware of the possible consequences of their action they might have behaved differently. One factor in the diminution of conflict may be an improvement in the parties’ abilities to identify the matters over which compromise may be possible. An important factor in the escalation of conflict is that as the parties become more separate, this ability declines and each side expects only hostile responses from the other. Table 1 is therefore interesting for the differences it shows between columns 1 and 2 on the one hand, and 3 and 4 on the other. Taking a difference of 10 percentage points as significant, it can be seen that the Chinese underestimated the strength of Malay ethnic preference in nine situations and overestimated it in one. The Malays underestimated the strength of Chinese ethnic preference in one situation and overestimated it in four. In the first three situations the Chinese respondents believed that a representative Malay fellow-citizen would be less influenced by considerations of social status relative to shared ethnicity than the Malay respondents believed, sometimes by a large margin. They seriously underestimated ethnic preference as an influence on Malay behaviour in the context of shopping, which is of some importance given the number of Chinese shopkeepers selling to Malay customers. They underestimated the reluctance of Malays to rent their houses temporarily to Chinese tenants, a reluctance which may stem from Malay religious beliefs and ideas of pollution which have no counterpart in Chinese beliefs. The one situation in which they overestimated the strength of Malay ethnic preference was in predicting whether Husin Ali’s ethnic preference would outweigh any feeling of personal obligation to his boss when others were trying to have him ousted in favour of a Malay. Maybe their overestimation was influenced by fear because the risks to them in these circumstances are so great. Malay predictions of how Tang Seng Seng would react in similar situations were much closer to Chinese predictions. It is possible that Malays may have projected their own sentiments of ethnic preference onto Chinese reactions regarding a mother’s advice about a marriage partner, about shopping and about employing a child-minder from the other group. They underestimated ethnic preference when it came to the adoption of a child, possibly because for some Malays bringing up the child as a Muslim would be more important than ethnic origin. It is sometimes said that members of a subordinated group learn to predict the likely reactions of members of the superordinate group because these reactions can be important to those who are at the receiving end. In Malaysia ethnic Chinese are politically Ethnic Conflict 491
subordinated to the Malays and as business people they need to take account of the tastes of their Malay customers. Nevertheless, the generalisation seems not to hold in the present case, where Malays predict Chinese reactions better than Chinese predict Malay reactions. The processes of mobilisation and bargaining between opposed parties are made up of common elements, whether they occur at the local or the national level, though the institutional structures which limit the possible outcomes can be very different. Nevertheless, it is often easier to identify these processes by examining what happens in local communities. F. G. Bailey (1996) has described his puzzlement over the change in a small Croatian town studied by anthropologists. In 1983 the people presented themselves as an amiable ethnic mix of Croats, Muslims, Serbs and a few Slovenes, intermarried, intermingled in the workplace, sharing neighbourhoods and neighbourliness, aware of ethnic differences and ready to joke about them, but apparently uninfluenced by ethnic prejudice. Before ten years had passed men were away fighting in one or another army; marriages had broken; households had been dispersed as members fled to whatever they claimed to be an ethnic homeland, or to refugee camps. Bailey did not understand, in the empathetic sense of that word, how good-natured neighbours could so quickly and so thoroughly be turned into demonised adversaries. So he re-examined from this standpoint notes from his fieldwork about forty years earlier in the Indian state of Orissa. There had been a dispute between caste groups (or jatis) in the village of Bisipara where almost everyone was either a brahmin, a warrior, a distiller, a herdsman, a potter, a washer- man or a weaver, and there was no exit option. The weavers were untouchables. They knew that with the passing of the Temple Entry Act in 1947 it had become an offence to bar Hindus from temples on the grounds of untouchability and decided to assert their new right. They notified the local police headquarters that on the occasion of a particular festival they would, as usual, take their offerings to a temple, but this time they would, like the clean castes, take them into the forechamber. The clean castes mounted a guard to prevent their doing so. The police arrived. Bailey believes that the police advised the weavers to seek a remedy through the courts and would have said that were there any more reports of trouble two constables would be stationed in the village. The clean castes punished the weavers by ceasing to employ them as musicians on festive occasions, but the case did not go to court and the previous pattern of relations was restored. Bailey interprets the strategy of the weavers as one of calculation in their choice of a particular temple, a peaceful protest, and in not pursuing legal action. They invited oversight of their dispute, but they did not press for intervention. Everyone knew that if a police unit were to be billeted on the village it would be a collective punishment. By demonstrating a capacity to press a shared interest they had presumably gained an enhanced self-respect at the price of a small material loss. To explain why the residents of Bisipara ‘did not behave like Serbs and Croats and 492
m i c h a e l ba n to n Muslims or like the Hutu and Tutsi’ Bailey described the multidimensionality of relations in the village and the interdependence of the groups. This was a necessary preliminary to his conclusion that ‘the main answer’ to his question was that ‘all those concerned … were accustomed to counting the cost’ of their actions (1996:156). They knew that if they performed services for others then others would perform services for them. They knew enough about their community to be able to calculate fairly accurately, and to know when to stop. The title of his book contends that civil society was facilitated by the villagers’ indifference to caste distinctions in some circumstances, and that to this extent they had domesticated the group prejudices (which he counts as ethnic). By 1994 relations had changed. Members of unclean castes had been murdered because one of them had entered a temple. The change probably reflected a reduction in the village’s relative isolation from the outside world. In the incident described by Bailey insiders invited outside attention; two outsiders who came to Bisipara to tell the villagers that it was unlawful to treat weavers less favourably made little impression upon daily life. Just as in some circumstances there is an exit option, so a reduction in isolation opens a vulnerability to the entry of influences that upset the equilibrium. According to many reports, the Hindus and Muslims in Ayodha had been able to co-exist without overt conflict until political mobilisers from elsewhere appeared on the scene. Ethnic preferences can be changed by the eloquence of mobilisers or by the messages of the mass media. Commentators agree that the highly selective use of television and radio to disseminate misleading accounts of the conduct of the groups in the former Yugoslavia did much to cause its break-up, while the influence of the radio in the Rwandan genocide has been widely acknowledged. Download 151.63 Kb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling