Chapter 1 the study of collocations


Download 0.8 Mb.
Pdf ko'rish
bet8/141
Sana08.01.2022
Hajmi0.8 Mb.
#246508
1   ...   4   5   6   7   8   9   10   11   ...   141
Bog'liq
colloca

1.3.3  The Structural Approach 
 
 
The structural approach consists of those linguists and researchers who 
suggest that collocation is influenced by structure, and collocations occur in 
patterns.  Therefore, the structural approach recommends that the study of 
collocations should include grammar. 
 
The Neo-Firthians' view of separating lexical analysis from grammatical 
analysis was criticised by Mitchell (1971), who argues for the "one-ness of 
grammar, lexis and meaning" (Mitchell 1971:43).  The interdependence of 
grammar and lexicon is evident from the fact that 'lexical particularities' derive 
their meaning not only from contextual extension of a lexical kind but also from 
the generalised grammatical patterns in which they appear (Mitchell 1971:48).  
For the study of collocations, Mitchell proposes that "collocations [which are 'of 
roots' rather than 'of words'] are to be studied within grammatical matrices" 
(Mitchell 1971:65).  In a group of word forms like 'drinks', 'drinker' and 
 
150


'drinking' Mitchell abstracts the common elements of each word form and 
labels that as 'root', e.g. /drink, and the associations of different roots, e.g. 
/drink- and /heav-, as 'collocations', e.g. 'heavy drinker', 'drink heavily' 
(Mitchell 1971:51).  Mitchell refers to the collocation 'heavy drinker' as an 
exemplification of the colligation 'adjective + agentive noun' (Mitchell 
1966:337).  The relationship between 'collocation' and 'colligation' is one of 
generality: 'colligations' are the generalised classes of associations and 
'collocations' are their particular members (Mitchell 1971:53). 
 
Mitchell's view that collocations are of roots rather than of words does 
not hold for every combination of roots.  For example, 'faint praise' is an 
acceptable English collocation, but not all combinations of the two roots, /faint- 
and /praise-, produce acceptable collocations: 'she was damned by faint praise' 
is acceptable, but 'he praised her faintly' is not. 
 
Matthews (1965) proposes another way of studying collocations within 
grammar.  He suggests enriching Chomsky's syntax with extra sets of rules that 
will account for the selectional restrictions on lexical items.  This approach 
deals with the syntagmatic relations along a string of lexical items, a 'kernel 
colligation' (p.38), rather than with individual collocational relations of pairs of 
words, but Matthews realises that such a description of the language involves  
double or triple the number of rules when compared to a description on the 
lines of Chomsky's syntax (Matthews 1965).  Matthews' theory suggested the 
study of syntagmatic relations, and consequently of collocation, along the lines 
of transformational grammar, but it was not developed any further.  
 
151


 
The influence of grammar on collocation was also discussed by 
Greenbaum (1970), (1974) who pointed out that certain instances of collocation 
require syntactic information.  For example 'much' collocates with 'prefer' when 
it is in a pre-verb position as in 'I much prefer a dry wine', but it does not 
collocate with 'prefer' in post-object position as in *'I prefer a dry wine much' 
(Greenbaum 1974:82).  Greenbaum suggests that the collacability of words (i.e. 
their potential co-occurrence with other lexical items) should be "tied" to 
syntax, and realises that there are certain lexical items that can occur only in 
certain syntactic relationships, e.g. 'His sincerity frightens us' but not 'We 
frighten his sincerity' (Greenbaum 1974:82).  Without reference to syntax, the 
notion of collacability becomes vacuous - virtually any two items can co-occur 
at a given arbitrary distance.  For example, 'sincerity' can collocate with 
'frighten’, but the acceptability of the combinations they produce can only be 
judged via syntax. 
 
The notion of language blocks and lexicalised sentence stems was 
introduced by Pawley and Syder (1983), who suggest that if a learner is going 
to achieve a native-like control of a language, then along with the rules of a 
generative grammar, she/he needs to "learn a means for knowing which of the 
well-formed sentences are native-like -- a way of distinguishing those sentences 
that are normal or unmarked from those that are unnatural or highly marked" 
(Pawley & Syder 1983:194).  Pawley and Syder propose a new way of 
examining native-like selection and fluency.  According to their approach, 
learners memorise a language in blocks, and a big portion of a native speaker's 
 
152


lexicon consists of "lexicalised sentence stems".  For example, an expression of 
apology like 'I'm sorry to keep you waiting' gives the sentence stem 'NP be-
TENSE sorry to keep-TENSE you waiting'; the constituents of this sentence are 
its 'inflections' and any additional constituents (e.g. 'all this time') are its 
'expansions' (Pawley & Syder 1983:210).  According to Pawley and Syder 
lexicalisation belongs to the domain of competence and a sentence stem can be 
lexicalised if it is a standard expression of the meaning in question in a 
Download 0.8 Mb.

Do'stlaringiz bilan baham:
1   ...   4   5   6   7   8   9   10   11   ...   141




Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling