Theoretical models of culture shock and adaptation in international students in higher education
Application of the acculturation model to international students
Download 308.88 Kb. Pdf ko'rish
|
Theoretical models of culture shock and adaptation in international students in higher education
Application of the acculturation model to international students
As we have seen, the affective, behavioural and cognitive aspects of adaptation are very much interrelated, but they are explored in sequence below, with particular reference to the literature on international students. Social and behavioural adaptation Bochner’s functional model of friendship networks (Bochner, McLeod, and Lin 1977; Ward, Bochner, and Furnham 2001) is still influential in contemporary studies of intercultural contact for student sojourners. Bochner suggests that such students tend to belong to three distinctive social networks, and each serves a particular psychological function. Through connections with their compatriots in the host country and, with increasing ease of long-distance communication, those remaining in the home country, international students might maintain their original cultural behaviour and values – this is the primary network. They also have interactions with host nation- als, such as home-based students, teachers and counsellors, through which they might learn a series of culturally relevant skills to facilitate their academic success. Thirdly, they might also have friendships with other non-compatriot foreign students, from which they derive mutual social support and enjoy some social recreational activities. These three are classified as mono- cultural, bi-cultural and multi-cultural friendship networks (Furnham 2004). Overseas students can benefit from interaction with host nationals socially, psychologically and academically. For example, a greater amount of interaction with host nationals has been asso- ciated with fewer academic problems (Pruitt 1978), fewer social difficulties (Ward and Kennedy 1993b), improved communication competency, and better general adaptation to life overseas (Zimmerman 1995). Overseas students participating in structured peer-pairing programmes (Westwood and Barker 1990; Abe, Talbot, and Geelhoed 1998), and spending more informal leisure time with their local peers (Pruitt 1978), have been found to have better social adjustment than those who did not. Additionally, contact and friendships with local students is associated with emotional benefits such as sojourner satisfaction (Rohrlich and Martin 1991) and lower levels of stress (Redmond and Bunyi 1993), and predicts better psychological adjustment (Searle and Ward 1990). Despite the benefits of host–sojourner interaction, the extent of this interaction is often limited (e.g. Nowack and Weiland 1998). Overseas students are generally most likely to report that their best friend is from the same culture (e.g. Bochner, McLeod, and Lin 1977). A number of research- ers use the concept of cultural distance (e.g. Furnham and Alibhai 1985) to interpret weak host– sojourner interaction. For example, Redmond and Bunyi’s (1993) study in a midwestern American university found that, among 644 international students, British, European and South American students were the best integrated, while Korean, Taiwanese and South-east Asian students were the least integrated. Fortunately, positive outcomes also stem from compatriot rela- tionships and links with non-compatriot foreign students. Greater co-national interaction is linked with stronger cultural identity (e.g. Ward and Searle 1991), and quantity and quality of interaction with non-compatriot foreign students is associated with perceived quality of social support (Kennedy 1999). Studies in Higher Education 71 Affective adaptation Social support also impacts on affective outcomes, although research on friendship networks places greater emphasis on the quantity and quality of actual support than the mere number of networks. Social support from both host and co-nationals can contribute to enhancement of student psychological well-being (e.g. Tanaka et al. 1997). Social support also alleviates ‘home- sickness’ (Hannigan 1997). However, the relationship between psychological adjustment and academic adaptation is not very clear. For example, how much do communication skills learned in friendly interactions with host students contribute to effective formal communication with host teachers, where patterns of affect might be quite different? Further research into how the psychological well-being and sociocultural adaptation of international students impede or enhance their academic success is needed. Cognitive adaptation The literature on cognitive aspects of acculturation in student sojourners has concentrated on inter-group perceptions and relations. Many international students perceive prejudice and discrimination during their interaction with host nationals (e.g. Sodowsky and Plake 1992). Some studies have even indicated that increased contact can in some cases lead to a sharpening of nega- tive inter-group stereotypes over time (e.g. Stroebe, Lenkert, and Jonas 1988). This is a reminder that contact theory, which hypothesises that increased contact improves inter-group perceptions and relations, might only work under certain circumstances. Bond’s (1986) study of local Chinese and American exchange students in Hong Kong revealed comparatively positive inter-group perceptions. His analysis included consideration of auto-stereotypes (in-group perceptions), hetero-stereotypes (out-group perceptions) and reflected stereotypes (how the out-group is perceived to view the in-group). Bond argued that the stereo- types accurately reflected significant differences in the behavioural characteristics of the two groups. Both Chinese and Americans perceived Chinese students as conservative and obedient, while both also perceived American students as questioning and independent. Such stereotypes might consistently influence interactions. Pratt (1991) commented that in America teachers are regarded as facilitators who promote learner autonomy, while in China students see teachers as authority figures, and are used to accepting academic assertions without questioning them. It seems that cross-cultural stereotypes, that is, cognitive aspects of the acculturation process for students, may have particularly important effects on the culture of learning, a concept proposed by Cortazzi and Jin (1997). The concept includes cultural beliefs and values about teaching and learning, and expectations about classroom behaviours. These ideas lead on to consideration of issues specifically concerned with what is going on in intercultural educational settings, and issues about student sojourners’ pedagogical adaptation. Download 308.88 Kb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling