You Can Learn to Remember: Change Your Thinking, Change Your Life pdfdrive com


Download 0.7 Mb.
Pdf ko'rish
bet19/64
Sana01.03.2023
Hajmi0.7 Mb.
#1238316
1   ...   15   16   17   18   19   20   21   22   ...   64
Bog'liq
@miltonbooks You Can Learn to Remember Change Your Thinking, Change

a good night’s sleep


People often ask me what I do to prepare for a memory competition. I
exercise my brain through memorization practice; and I make sure that my
circulation is at its best through physical exercise. Just as importantly,
immediately before competing, I make sure that I have a good night’s sleep.
First, in the afternoon of the day prior to a contest, I run at least four
miles – which means that, once the adrenaline rush of the run has faded, I
am physically tired. Second, I take some ginkgo biloba – which improves
memory. And third, I practice a presleep meditation, to quieten my
anxieties (even World Champ ions get nervous), and to put me in the frame
of mind for deep sleep.
memory and learning
T
here can be no learning without memory. A wealth of psychological research
has shown that for both animals and humans memory forms a crucial part of the
learning process. Even the acquisition of apparently basic skills, such as when a
baby learns to crawl, would be impossible without the existence of procedural
(or implicit) memory.
In the early nineteenth century, the German philosopher Hermann
Ebbinghaus demonstrated that the amount of information we retain depends
upon the amount of time we spend learning (the “totaltime hypothesis”). He also
realized that it is more effective to break up the total learning time into short
periods (of between fifteen and forty-five minutes), separated by five-or ten-
minute breaks. This is the “distribution-practice effect”, and it works partly
because of a phenomenon called reminiscence – the way in which our memory
of something actually improves steadily over a period of several minutes after
we have stopped learning it. Reminiscence is probably a result of the memory
traces gradually strengthening. The timescale for reminiscence varies with the
type of learning: somewhat surprisingly, perhaps, our memory of a photograph is
strongest one and a half minutes after studying it, while our memory of a manual
skill is strongest around ten minutes after first practising it. Distributed learning
increases our number of reminiscence periods. Also, when we learn blocks of
information, the memories that we create interfere with each other, and regular
intervals of rest lessen this effect. Another learning strategy, applied
unconsciously, is chunking.
In 1956 American psychologist George Miller noted that the short-term


memory seems able to hold only about seven items at a time, placing an upper
limit on the powers of retention – if we look at a scattering of marbles on the
floor, we will only be able to hold in our minds the positions of a maximum of
seven of them before we become confused. Miller speculated that the short-term
memory can hold vast amounts of information, provided that information is
organized into no more than seven coherent “chunks”. The brain seems to do this
automatically – for example, as children, we did not learn the alphabet as an
unbroken string of 26 letters, but used rhythm and inflection to divide it into
something similar to abcd/efg/hijk/lmnop/qrs/tuv/wxyz – seven manageable
units.

Download 0.7 Mb.

Do'stlaringiz bilan baham:
1   ...   15   16   17   18   19   20   21   22   ...   64




Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling