1 language learning in early childhood preview
Download 441.06 Kb. Pdf ko'rish
|
Pedagogía
American Sign
Language (ASL) make use of grammatical markers to indicate such things as time (for example, past tense) and number. These markers are expressed through specific hand or body movements. Elissa Newport (1990) and her colleagues studied the ability of deaf users of ASL to produce and comprehend grammatical markers. They compared Native signers (who were exposed to ASL from birth), Early signers (who began using ASL between four and six years of age), and Late signers (who began learning ASL after age 12). They found no difference between the groups in some aspects of their use of ASL, for example in vocabulary knowledge. However, on tests focusing on grammatical markers, the Native group used the markers more consistently than the Early group who, in turn, used them more consistently than the Late group. The researchers concluded that their study supports the hypothesis that there is a critical period for first language acquisition, whether that language is oral or gestural. There has been controversy regarding the importance of exposing deaf children to sign language as early as possible. Some parents and educators, as well as speech and language therapists, have argued that early exposure to sign language could interfere with children’s development of oral language. However, current research suggests that even for children who are given cochlear implants and thus have greater access to oral language, exposure to sign language adds to—rather than interferes with—their overall development of language knowledge and skill (Humphries et al., 2012). Another line of research that has given new insight into the importance of early language experience comes from studies of ‘international adoptees’. These are children who were adopted at an early age by families who did not speak the language the child had heard during infancy. In their review of studies of international adoptees, Johanne Paradis, Fred Genesee, and Martha Crago (2011) concluded that cognitive and linguistic outcomes were generally very positive. Some comparisons of the adoptees’ language with that of children the same age who had always heard the same language showed that subtle differences persist even after several years, but these are not the kinds of differences that most people would notice, nor do they prevent the adoptees from successfully using their new language. Here again, of course, one cannot know whether something other than a late exposure to the language spoken in the adoptive environment also contributed to these subtle differences. It is clear, therefore, that late exposure does not mean that a child cannot successfully learn and use a new language. Nevertheless, with continuing research on children’s linguistic behaviours and intuitions, as well as the studies of infants’ speech perception that we saw above, there is growing evidence that language acquisition begins at birth, and possibly even before, as the child’s brain is shaped by exposure to the language(s) in the environment. We will return to the issue of early and late language experiences in discussions of bilingualism and second language learning. Download 441.06 Kb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling