M. Iriskulov, A. Kuldashev a course in Theoretical English Grammar Tashkent 2008
participants and their syntactic expression in the clause
Download 1.52 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
Ingliz tili nazariy grammatikasi.M.Irisqulov.2008.
participants and their syntactic expression in the clause. One –participant clause (intransitive) presents a situation as involving only one participant, which is an Experiencer, Mover or Patient. There are three types of 155 intransitives: unergatives, e.g.: The child slept., unaccusatives, e.g.: The building collapsed., middles, e.g.: The car drives smoothly. The poem doesn’t translate. I don’t photograph very well. Two- participant clause (transitive) prototypically involves the transfer of energy from an Agent (the subject) to a Patient (the object), e.g.: The farmer shot the rabbit. The prototypical transitive clause can also be made passive, e.g.: The rabbit was shot by the farmer. A remarkable fact about the schema for a prototypical transitive clause is that it accommodates all manner of relations between entities. The following examples exhibit this fact, though exhibiting fewer and fewer characteristics of a transitive interaction, e.g.: I remember the event. My car burst a tyre. The road follows the river. Joe resembles his grandfather. The non-prototypical status of these transitives is proven by the fact that they cannot be made passive. Three-participant clause (double-object clause) is a clause where a second post-verbal object is obligatory, its presence determines the existence of the clause as such, e.g.: I’ll mail you the report. I’ll bake you a cake. The three participants are the Agent, the thing that undergoes changes at the hands of the Agent, and the person which benefits from the change (Beneficiary). Characteristic of this clause type is that the Beneficiary is construed as the Patient of the interaction and it appears immediately after the verb, as the verb’s object (it means that “my” action directly affects “you”, in that “you” come to receive the report). The clause profiles the relation between the Agent and Beneficiary by means of placing the Beneficiary immediately after the verb. The sentence renders the idea of “possessivity”. The same situation can be conceptualized in an alternative way, e.g.: I’ll mail the report to you. I’ll bake a cake for you. The clause bears the intermediary status between the prototypical two-participant clause and prototypical three-participant clause. It profiles the relation between the Agent and Patient. The sentence renders the idea of “path”. In the end it should be noted that different types of processes (event concepts) appear to be “packed” into two basic syntactic configurations: transitive and intransitive constructions. It becomes possible due to the fact that the subject and object can instantiate not only their prototypical use, the Agent and Patient, but also other semantic roles. This mechanism is the basis of alternative conceptualizations (imagery) of situations of the real world in syntactic forms. The c l a s s i f i c a t i o n of larger syntactic units - c l a u s e c o m b i n a t i o n s (c l a u s e c o m p l e x e s) - is based on the criterion of the degree of integration between clauses J.R. Taylor distinguishes minimal integration, coordination, subordination, complementation, clause fusion which reveals the highest degree of integration. Clause complexes of minimal integration. Two clauses are simply juxtaposed, with no overt linking, e.g.: I came, I saw, I conquered. The clauses are in sequential relation to each other – the first mentioned was the first to occur. Clause complexes of coordination. Each clause could in principle stand alone as an independent conceptualization. The clauses are linked by means of words such as and, but, or, e.g.: She prefers fish, and/but I prefer pasta. A slightly 156 higher degree of integration is possible if both clauses share the same subject, e.g.: I went up to him and asked the way. Clause complexes of subordination. Here, there are two clauses, but one is understood in terms of a particular semantic relation (temporal, causal, etc.) to each other. Typical subordinators are after, if, whenever, although. Clause complexes based on complementation. Complementation represents a closer integration of clauses, in that one clause functions as a participant in another. There are different syntactic forms that a complement clause can take. A complement clause functions as the subject or the object of the main verb. The complement clause may appear as: - an infinitive without to, e.g.: I saw them break into the house; - “to”-infinitive, e.g.: To finish it in time was impossible. I advise you to wait a while. I want to go there myself; - “ing”-form of the verb, e.g.: I avoided meeting them. I can’t imagine him saying that; - subordinate clause, introduced by that or question words e.g.: I hope that we will see each other again soon, I wonder what we should do. Clause fusions represent the highest degree of integration. It occurs when two clauses fuse into a single clause, e.g.: These cars are expensive to repair. One could “unpack” this sentence into two independent clauses, designating two different processes: “someone repairing the cars” and “this process is expensive”. In the example the two clausal conceptions have fused into one. We characterize the cars as “expensive” with respect to a certain process. (For details see: Taylor J.R. Cognitive Grammar. 2002). Summing it all up, it is necessary to mention that sentence classifications proposed by different linguists within a cognitive approach are aimed at grouping sentences on the basis of their formal properties in relation to the concepts they represent as well as the conceptual mechanisms which enable the creation of different types of sentences (cognitive functions of Figure and Ground in L.Talmy’s conception or operations of conceptual integration in J.R. Taylor’s typology). It is evident that such like classifications bear the status of more unified theories of sentences compared to the classifications introduced within the traditional approaches to syntax. Traditional syntax profiles the formal characteristics of syntactic units which results in the strict division: “the simple sentence, the composite sentence: the complex and the compound sentences”. Sentence classifications proposed within a cognitive approach profile the concepts represented by syntactic constructions, conceptual mechanisms which determine the production of different types of sentence and which in the most general sense reflect the basic conceptualization processes. “Cognitive” classifications, by their nature, are more likely to show that the distinctive features of sentence types form a continuum rather than discreet categories which reflect the work of human mind. |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling