Non-Native Perception and Interpretation of English Intonation


Download 99.4 Kb.
Pdf ko'rish
bet4/9
Sana19.06.2023
Hajmi99.4 Kb.
#1623216
1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9
Bog'liq
parisberg, atoye

Nordic Journal of African Studies 
vitiate the findings. They were all native-speakers of Yoruba from the 
southwestern part of Nigeria. The choice of native-speakers of Yoruba enabled 
one to avoid potential problems caused by the influence of diverse mother 
tongues.
Yoruba is a tone language that is spoken in the southwestern part of Nigeria. 
According to Laniran and Clements (2003: 204), Yoruba operates “a register tone 
system with three distinctive tone levels, high (H), mid (M) and low (L)”. Though 
it is traditionally analysed as a tone language, native speakers of Yoruba appear to 
make use of intonation to discriminate between syntactically unmarked 
affirmative and interrogative sentences, as found out by Atoye.
The subjects were all third-year university undergraduates of the Department 
of English, Obafemi Awolowo University in Nigeria. Prior to the experiment, they 
had been exposed to the traditional analysis of intonation and its functions through 
the English Phonetics and Phonology courses. They therefore constituted a highly 
homogenous socio-linguistic group with regards to such variables as age, 
education, exposure to and training in English phonetics.
3.3 T
EST 
D
ESIGN AND 
A
DMINISTRATION
Each subject was given a sheet of paper on which the ten English test sentences 
had been arranged in pairs. The members of each pair were lexically and 
syntactically identical on paper since the intonation that differentiated them was 
not indicated in the sentences given to them. Commas and other intra-sentence 
punctuation marks were also avoided so as not to give away the intonation 
contours on the sentences. The subjects had, therefore, to rely absolutely on their 
auditory perception of the intonation contours of the pre-recorded sentences 
played back to them on an audio cassette-recorder. 
The sentences were played back to the subjects using a different intonation 
contour on each member of a pair. The intonation used on each sentence gave it a 
different meaning from the other member of the pair. The two sentences in each 
pair therefore constituted an intonation minimal pair as they differed only in the 
intonation employed on them. As suggested by de Bot and Mailfert (1982: 76), 
“such recorded minimal pairs can be a useful technique” for teachers to show their 
students that intonation plays an essential role in communication.
The subjects were asked to listen carefully to the five pre-recorded pairs of 
sentences played back to them and to perform two tasks. For the first task, they 
were asked to indicate, in the space between each pair on the given piece of paper, 
whether they perceived any difference between the intonation contours of the two 
sentences. For the second task, they were asked to state the meaning of each 
sentence, using the intonation pattern with which it was said as a guide. It was 
expected that the subjects would indicate the same meaning for the two members 
in a pair if they did not perceive any variation in their intonation contours or if 
they did not think that the difference in intonation was linguistically significant. 
32


Non-Native Perception and Interpretation 
The multiple-choice answer format suggested by Cruz-Ferreira (1989) was 
discarded in the present case because of its inherent problems. Such a format, like 
any multiple-choice objective question format, encourages guesswork on the part 
of the respondents and could lead to a false rating of their comprehension level. 
This is particularly so in Cruz-Ferreira’s proposal in which one of the three 
suggested answers is “in fact not a possible interpretation”, thus making 
guesswork highly profitable, as was that author’s finding in an earlier experiment 
in which the format was used (Cruz-Ferreira 1983). In addition, the use of that 
format would inadvertently limit the possible range of interpretations that the 
subjects could otherwise have given, some of which may have escaped the 
researcher’s own attention or even his or her imagination, and which may have 
been right, nonetheless. It also presupposes that only one meaning is associated 
with each intonation contour, in spite of the criticism of such a monolithic 
interpretation of intonation reviewed earlier in the present paper. The open-ended 
or ‘free’ format, adopted in the present experiment, led the subjects to actually do 
the interpretation, all by themselves, thus encouraging originality and diversity in 
their interpretation and making them fully involved as participants in a simulated 
communicative event.
The test sentences, some of which were adopted or adapted from the existing 
literature on the subject, are presented in 3.4 below, as they were printed on the 
paper given to the subjects, without any intra-sentence punctuation or indication 
of tone group boundaries or direction of pitch flow.
To give the reader a fair idea of what was played back to the subjects, the test 
sentences are presented in 3.5 with a slanting line indicating each tone-group 
boundary, which also indicated a pause and a change in pitch direction. The 
standard meaning of each of the test sentences is also paraphrased in 3.6.

Download 99.4 Kb.

Do'stlaringiz bilan baham:
1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9




Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling