Second Language Learning and Language Teaching
Plural ‘-s’. The easiest morpheme for them was the plural ‘-s’, getting ‘Girls go’. 2
Download 1.11 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
cook vivian second language learning and language teaching
1 Plural ‘-s’. The easiest morpheme for them was the plural ‘-s’, getting ‘Girls go’.
2 Progressive ‘-ing’. Next easiest was the word ending ‘-ing’ in present continuous forms like ‘going’, ‘Girls going’. 3 Copula forms of ‘be’. Next came the use of ‘be’ as a copula, that is, as a main verb in the sentence (‘John is happy’) rather than as an auxiliary used with another verb (‘John is going’). Changing the sentence slightly gets ‘Girls are here’. 4 Auxiliary form of ‘be’. After this came the auxiliary forms of ‘be’ with ‘-ing’, yielding ‘Girls are going’. 5 Definite and indefinite articles ‘the’ and ‘a’. Next in difficulty came the definite and indefinite articles ‘the’ and ‘a’, enabling the learners to produce ‘The girls go’ or ‘A girl go’. 6 Irregular past tense. Next were the irregular English past tenses such as ‘came’ and ‘went’, that is, those verbs that do not have an ‘-ed’ ending pronounced in the usual three ways / d /, / t / or / id /, ‘played’, ‘learnt’ and ‘waited’, as in ‘The girls went’. 7 Third person ‘-s’. Next came the third person ‘-s’ used with verbs, as in ‘The girl goes’. Learning and teaching different types of grammar 26 Content words Structure words are in the dictionary: ‘book’ are in the grammar: ‘the’ exist in large numbers, 615,000 in the are limited in number, say, 220 in Oxford English Dictionary English vary in frequency: ‘book’ versus ‘honved’ are high frequency: ‘to’, ‘the’, ‘I’ are used more in written language are used more in spoken language are more likely to be preceded by a are less likely to be preceded by a pause pause in speech in speech consist of nouns ‘glass’; verbs ‘move’; consist of prepositions ‘to’; articles ‘a’; adjectives ‘glossy’, etc. pronouns ‘he’, etc. are always pronounced and spelt the vary in pronunciation for emphasis, same: ‘look’ / lυk/ etc.: ‘have’ /h v, hɘv, ɘv, v/ have a fixed stress or stresses: ‘pilot’ are stressed for emphasis, etc.: ‘the’ / ði ~ ðɘ/ have more than two letters: ‘eye’, ‘Ann’ can consist of one or two letters: ‘I’, ‘an’ are pronounced with an initial voiceless are pronounced with an initial voiced ‘th’: ‘theory’ / θ/ ‘th’: ‘there’ / ð/ can always be invented: ‘cyberpunk’ can seldom be invented Table 2.1 Content words and structure words 8 Possessive ‘’s’. Most difficult of the eight endings was the ‘’s’ ending used with nouns to show possession, as in ‘The girl’s book’. The sequence from 1 to 8 mirrors the order of difficulty for the L2 learners Dulay and Burt studied. They had least difficulty with plural ‘-s’ and most diffi- culty with possessive ‘’s’. The interesting discovery was the similarities between the L2 learners. It was not just Spanish-speaking children who have a sequence of difficulty for the eight grammatical morphemes. Similar orders have been found for Japanese children and for Korean adults (Makino, 1980; Lee, 1981), though not for one Japanese child (Hakuta, 1974). The first language does not seem to make a crucial difference: all L2 learners have much the same order. This was quite surprising in that people had thought that the main problem in acquiring gram- mar was transfer from the first language; now it turned out that learners had the same types of mistake whatever the first language they spoke. The other surprise was that it did not seem to matter if the learners were children or adults; adults have roughly the same order as children (Krashen et al., 1976). It does not even make much difference whether or not they are attending a language class (Larsen- Freeman, 1976). There is a strong similarity between all L2 learners of English, whatever the explanation may be. This research with grammatical morphemes was the first to demonstrate the common factors of L2 learners so clearly. While grammatical morphemes petered out as a topic of research in the 1990s, it was the precursor of much research to do with the acquisition of grammatical inflections such as past tense ‘-ed’ which is still common today. Yet there are still things to learn from this area. Muhammad Hannan (2004), for instance, used it to find a sequence of acquisition for Bengali-speaking children in East London, as mentioned in Chapter 1. At the age of 5, they knew only ‘-ing’, as in ‘looking’; by 6 they had added past tense / t / ‘looked’; by 7 irregular past tenses such as ‘went’, and regular / d / ‘played’; by 8 past participles ‘-en’ ‘been’; by 9 the only persistent problem was with ‘zero’ past ‘hit’. Clearly these children made a consistent pro- gression for grammatical morphemes over time. This type of research brought important confirmation of the idea of the learner’s independent language, interlanguage. Learners from many backgrounds seemed to be creating the same kind of grammar for English out of what they heard, and were passing through more or less the same stages of acquisition. They were reacting in the same way to the shared experience of learning English. While the first language made some difference, its influence was dwarfed by what the learners had in common. Indeed, at one point Dulay and Burt (1973) dramatically claimed that only 3 per cent of learners’ errors could be attributed to interference from the first language. While later research has seldom found such a low inci- dence, nevertheless it became clear that much of the learning of a second lan- guage was common to all L2 learners rather than being simply transfer from their first language. One of the best demonstrations of the independence of interlanguage came from a research programme that investigated the acquisition of five second lan- guages by adult migrant workers in Europe, known as the ESF (European Science Foundation) project. Researchers found a basic grammar that all L2 learners shared, which had three simple rules; a sentence may consist of: ● a noun phrase followed by a verb, optionally followed by another noun phrase ‘girl take bread’; Structure words, morphemes and sequences of acquisition 27 L2 learners not only have an interlanguage grammar, they have the same interlan- guage grammar, regardless of the language they are learning. In other words, all that teachers can actually expect from learners after a year or so is a sparse gram- mar having these three rules; whatever the teacher may try to do, this may be what the learners achieve. Download 1.11 Mb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling