Technical Translation: Usability Strategies for Translating Technical Documentation
Download 2.88 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
byrne jody technical translation usability strategies for tr
- Bu sahifa navigatsiya:
- Verbal Protocols
Interactive Observation
Interactive observation is a type of indirect observation where the part of the system or computer is played by a member of the evaluation team. Commonly known as the “Wizard of Oz” (Faulkner 1998:122), this ap- proach makes users think that they are using a real system, but in reality all of the system’s responses and actions are performed by a human. This method is effective in that it does not require a fully functional version of the system and it can be implemented reasonably cheaply in comparison to the expense of producing a fully-functioning system. However, this ap- proach is formative and is more suited to situations where an actual soft- ware system is being produced. The effort required to create an interface, in terms of design and labour coupled with the additional staff requirements to conduct the experiment make this approach difficult to implement. In any case, this approach is of limited applicability to documentation as it is aimed at investigating the way the software interface works. 184 Interaction logging tools operate in a similar manner except that they record the entire interaction in real-time. What makes interaction logging Approaches to Empirical Evaluation Verbal Protocols clamations and other information which may arise during the course of an experiment. One particular variety of verbal protocol is the think-aloud protocol which involves users saying what they are thinking, feeling, plan- ning etc. as they perform tasks and use the interface. This can provide a valuable insight into what users want to do, what they think they are doing and what their responses are when something unexpected occurs. It is also possible to gain an insight into how users remember commands, plan and execute tasks and how they recover from errors (Preece 1993:113). Verbal protocols of this type are generally used in conjunction with audio or video recording (Preece 1994:621). Although think-aloud protocols (TAP) are ideal for formative usability testing where the wealth of qualitative data they can provide is extremely useful in understanding the nature of the interaction, in the case of purely summative evaluations where, for example, the speed at which users work is being measured, TAP is less applicable, chiefly because summative evaluations require quantitative data. It can also be argued that TAP may hinder evaluation rather than aid it. This can be attributed to a number of factors. Firstly, as we discussed in Chapter 3 the human cognitive system can realistically deal only with one response to a stimulus at a time even though it can process several inputs or stimuli. Indeed, the process of divid- solving and verbalising thoughts where high levels of accuracy are required for both. Similarly, it is also held that the very act of putting into words what it is a user is doing will affect the way the user performs the task (Dix 1998:427; Downton 1991:334). While many agree that this double-tasking will degrade performance on both tasks, there is some conflicting evidence that the think-aloud protocol may actually improve performance of the task. If this were proven to be true, it could be because the verbalisation process focuses a user’s mind on the task and helps users rationalise the task better. Nevertheless, the additional strain of performing two complex and demanding tasks such as putting thoughts into words can result in lower performance and some users will simply be unable to verbalise their thoughts (Preece 1994:622). There is also the problem of silence caused by the fact that users are ei- ther unaccustomed to thinking out loud or because all of their concentra- tion is being devoted to performing the task; some users may simply forget to speak. This problem is discussed by Dumas & Redish (1993:278-281) 185 Verbal protocols are spoken records of users’ comments, observations, ex- ing one’s attention equally between two tasks is unreliable at best, but extremely difficult when performing two complex tasks such as problem- Assessing Usability who point out that while some people have no problems whatsoever in producing an “unedited stream of consciousness”, others either mumble or do not speak. The authors make the interesting point that users need to be taught how to think out loud and that they may need to be reminded to do so. This in itself can become a source of stress for users who may already feel pressurised as a result of the tasks (Preece 1994:622). In view of these problems, retrospective or post-event protocols are sometimes used to elicit verbal data from users. Instead of commenting on their actions while they perform them, users are shown a video of the experiment and are asked to comment on their activities. This approach pro- duces different results in terms of the type of information users provide. Ac- cording to Preece, users tend to rationalise or interpret their actions or even justify them (Preece 1994:623). Rather than simply stating what they were doing or thinking, users tend to explain what they are doing and why. Dumas & Redish, however, do make the point that retrospective proto- cols frequently yield more suggestions as to how to improve the interface as compared to think-aloud protocols (Dumas & Redish 1993:279). However, we are not interested in using the evaluation to improve quality, merely to assess it. It is clear from the preceding paragraphs that observational methods are extremely useful in gathering comprehensive information on the way users work with an interface. While these methods produce large volumes of data which can be more difficult to analyse, the sheer detail and insight they provide more than compensates for this (Preece 1993:119). By using indi- rect observation we avoid such negative effects as the Hawthorne effect and we are provided with a permanent record of the experiment. In order to ensure that participants' task performance during the experiments is as rep- resentative of real-life as possible, think-aloud protocols are to be avoided as they can affect the way tasks are performed. Retrospective protocols are of limited use and are really only of benefit when the purpose of the evalua- tion is improvement rather than quantification; in the case of this study, we are concerned with the latter. Download 2.88 Mb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling