An Introduction to Applied Linguistics
partial because either only part of the discourse has been heard so far, or only
Download 1.71 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
Norbert Schmitt (ed.) - An Introduction to Applied Linguistics (2010, Routledge) - libgen.li
partial because either only part of the discourse has been heard so far, or only part has been comprehended. Inferencing is more subtle and in a sense operates at a higher level: ‘everything is comprehensible, but there is meaning to the discourse that exceeds the understanding of each of the utterances or part of it. Adding these together, only by inferencing will the whole be comprehended’ 185 Listening (Mendelsohn, 1994: 105). Inferencing can be thought of as ‘listening between the lines’. Listening Skills The convention is to refer to ‘the four language skills’, but it is clear that each of these comprises a large number of sub-skills, whose value and relevance vary from one situation to another. Richards (1983) was one of the first to categorize the sub-skills required in different listening situations; he came up with 33 micro- skills for conversational listening (CL) and a further 18 for academic listening to lectures (AL). His analysis raises a number of interesting questions, of which we will briefly mention two. The first question is: What is the relationship between conversational and academic micro-skills? Richards implied it was incremental: that all conversational listening micro-skills are required for academic listening, but that certain more specialized academic listening micro-skills (such as ‘coping with different styles of lecturing’) are required only in the lecture hall – making a possible academic listening total of 51 micro-skills. On the other hand, some micro-skills listed in both sets, such as ‘identifying and reconstructing topics’ (CL) and ‘identifying the lecture topic and following its development’ (AL), appear to rely on the same comprehension processes. Secondly, there is the question of the internal ordering of the micro-skills. Richards used the term ‘taxonomies’ of listening skill, which implied that the relationship within each set was hierarchical. That leads us to ask whether the successful use of some micro-skills depends on prior success in using others. Presumably it does; for example, one can hardly deduce the meaning of a word (conversational listening micro-skill 12) until you have distinguished its boundaries, for example, recognized its phonological form from the rest of the speech stream (conversational listening micro-skill 8). Richards’ analysis has been extremely influential in helping language teachers to distinguish and prioritize the components of different types of listening, and his article is still widely cited in discussion of materials design. His micro-skill taxonomies were later reshaped and developed by Rost (1990), who emphasized the importance of identifying ‘clusters’ of listening micro- skills. As Rost pointed out, his proposal for clustered practice also reflected wider doubts as to whether learning a complex skill can be effectively helped by step-by-step practice of its components, and whether learners can re- synthesize them in actual use. Rost’s clusters of micro-skills are shown in Table 11.1, which makes clear his key distinction between ‘enabling skills’ (those employed in order to perceive what the speaker is saying and to interpret what they intend to mean) and ‘enacting skills’ (those employed to respond appropriately to the message). Rost’s (1990) division of listening into perception, interpretation, and response shows parallels with the information processing models we mentioned earlier: ‘Perception, Parsing and Understanding’ (Anderson, 1985) and ‘Identify, Search, File and Use’ (Brown, 1995a). It helps us to distinguish between the levels of comprehension success and to pinpoint failure. In the Hands-on Activity at the end of this chapter we will be using Rost’s (1990) micro-skill clusters (see above) to help us categorize areas of success and failure in an individual L2 learner’s understanding of a listening text. 186 An Introduction to Applied Linguistics ENABLING SKILLS Perception 1. Recognizing prominence within utterances, including • Discriminating sounds in words, especially phonemic contrasts • Discriminating strong and weak forms, phonetic change at word boundaries • Identifying use of stress and pitch (information units, emphasis, etc.) Interpretation 2. Formulating content sense of an utterance, including • Deducing the meaning of unfamiliar words • Inferring implicit information • Inferring links between propositions 3. Formulating a conceptual framework linking utterances, including • Recognizing discourse markers (clarifying, contrasting) • Constructing a theme over a stretch of discourse • Predicting content • Identifying elements that help you to form an overall schema • Maintaining and updating the context 4. Interpreting (possible) speaker intentions, including • Identifying an ‘interpersonal frame’ speaker-to-hearer • Monitoring changes in prosody and establishing (in)consistencies • Noting contradictions, inadequate information, ambiguities • Differentiating between fact and opinion ENACTING SKILLS 5. Making an appropriate response (based on 1–4 above), including • Selecting key points for the current task • Transcoding information into written form (for example, notes) • Identifying which points need clarification • Integrating information with that from other sources • Providing appropriate feedback to the speaker. (adapted from Rost (1990: 152–3)) Table 11.1 Micro-skill clusters in listening comprehension Listening Strategies Interest in strategy use and strategy instruction derives from research over the years into ways of facilitating language learning (Rubin, 1975; Wenden and Rubin, 1987; O’Malley and Chamot, 1990; Oxford, 1990; see also Chapter 10, Focus on the Language Learner: Styles, Strategies, and Motivation). Chamot (1987: 71) provides a good basic definition of learning strategies: ‘techniques, approaches, or deliberate actions that students take in order to facilitate the learning and recall of both linguistic and content area information’. Research into strategy use has led to the development of a ‘strategy-based approach’ to teaching listening comprehension (Mendelsohn, 1994). As we stated in the opening section, people are usually not conscious of how they listen in their first language unless they encounter difficulty. So what L2 learners need to do when listening is to make conscious use of the strategies they unconsciously use in their first language. Learning strategies are usually divided into meta-cognitive, cognitive and social/ affective strategies – a tripartite classification developed by O’Malley, Chamot, 187 Listening Stewner-Manzanares, Kupper and Russo (1985). The table above brings together the listening strategies that researchers have identified in L2 contexts. Skilful listeners use these strategies in combination, varying their use according to the needs of the specific situation – a process that has been described as ‘orchestration’ (Vandergrift 2003). Research into L2 listening strategy use has recently tended to focus on meta-cognitive strategies and the work done by Vandergrift, Goh, Mareschal and Tafaghodfari (2006) is of particular potential interest for the language teacher. This team developed, tested and validated a Meta-cognitive Awareness Listening Questionnaire (MALQ), which they trialled with L2 listeners in Canada, Singapore and the Netherlands. When the listeners’ self-report responses were correlated with their performances on listening tests, five factors were found to be associated with success in listening: ‘Problem-solving’ (guessing and monitoring those guesses); ‘Planning and Elaboration’ (preparing for listening and assessing success); avoiding ‘Mental Translation’; ‘Person Knowledge’ (confidence or anxiety, self-perception as a listener); and ‘Directed Attention’ (ways of concentrating on aspects of the task). Cognitive Meta-cognitive Social/affective Predicting / Inferencing • from the text • from the voice • from the body language • between discourse parts Elaboration • from personal experience • from world knowledge • from academic learning • from imagination Contextualization Imagery Summarization • mental • physical (notes) Translation Repetition Transfer from other language(s) Deduction Fixation • stopping to think about spelling • stopping to think about meaning • stopping to memorize Planning • advance organization • self-management Comprehension monitoring • confirming comprehension • identifying words not understood Directed attention • concentrating • persevering despite problems Selective attention • listening for familiar words • listening for the overall message • noticing the information structure • noticing repetition and reformulation • listening to specific parts Evaluation • checking interpretation against predictions • checking interpretation against knowledge • checking interpretation against context Questioning (two-way tasks) • asking for clarification • asking for repetition • using comprehension check Cooperation • working with other learners Anxiety reduction • encouraging yourself • comparing yourself with others • focusing on success Relaxation • using physical techniques • using visualization (Based on Goh 2002; Vandergrift 2003; and Kondo and Yang 2004.) Table 11.2. Listening strategies 188 An Introduction to Applied Linguistics How do we Gain Insights into Listening? Settings Experiments Experimental investigation has tended to concentrate on quantifiable aspects, such as the effects of prosody on speech recognition. From experiments we know that the characteristic patterning of speech in our L1 provides a metrical template that influences the way we process L2 speech. Speakers of French, for example, have been shown to rely on syllable patterns to segment the stream of spoken French, whereas speakers of English use stress patterns rather than syllable patterns to parse L1 speech (Cutler, 2001). Delabatie and Bradley (1995) found that maintaining these unconscious L1 metrical habits caused listeners problems up to relatively advanced levels of L2 proficiency. Experimental approaches are well-suited to assessing the effects of other quantitative features on L2 comprehension. In the case of speed of speaking, for example, the absolute rate of speaking seems to matter less than the position and frequency of pauses. However, since real-life listening occurs in a specific social and cognitive context, other approaches are necessary to study the processes of making sense of ‘meaning’, as opposed to recognizing form. Pedagogic tasks As we mentioned earlier, the literature on L2 listening has tended to focus on pedagogic settings, such as the lecture theatre (Chaudron and Richards, 1986, Flowerdew, 1994; Vidal 2003; Carrell, Dunkel and Mollaun, 2004). Among the main findings have been the beneficial effects on L2 comprehension of content redundancy, pausing, macro-level signposting and visual support. Foreign language classroom studies that have explored listening within an interactive setting (Yule and Powers, 1994, Lynch, 1997) have emphasized the additional complexities for the listener of having, in Rost’s terms, to ‘enact’ a response, by contributing relevantly and coherently at an appropriate point in the discourse. Test performances Researchers with access to candidates’ performances in listening in worldwide tests such as IELTS and TOEFL have been able to investigate listening skills on a very large scale. Tsui and Fullilove (1998) sampled 150,000 item performances by Chinese learners of English to investigate whether skill in bottom-up processing (rather than top-down) makes some listeners more successful than others. They compared performances on questions where the correct answer matched the likely content schema with items where the answer conflicted with the schema. Candidates who got the correct answer for non-matching schema items tended to be more skilled listeners; presumably, the less skilled could rely on guessing for the matching items, but not for non-matching ones. Bottom-up processing seemed therefore to be more important than top-down processing in discriminating between candidates’ listening performance. Ethnographic research Although test-based studies have the advantage of scale, what they gain in terms of statistical robustness has to be weighed against what they may lose in 189 Listening lifelikeness. Observation of actual listening behaviour – such as misunderstandings in conversation – can yield rich data for analysis. One context in which such misunderstandings are common is the daily life of immigrants or migrant workers, as shown by an extensive ethnographic study of immigrant listeners in five European countries (Bremer, Roberts, Vasseur, Simonot and Broeder, 1996). The study was based on real-life or naturalistic encounters with native speakers in a variety of gate-keeper roles, such as job interviewer and social security officer. In some cases, misunderstandings were found to have primarily linguistic causes, but in most cases the miscommunications the research team analysed were rooted in non-shared expectations of members of different cultures, rather than simply in gaps in linguistic knowledge. We will be analysing just such an intercultural misunderstanding in the second Hands-on Activity at the end of this chapter. Methods The fact that comprehension occurs largely unobserved means that it can be very difficult to establish the ‘process’ by which listeners have reached their interpretations, even if we have evidence of the ‘product’ (what they understood). However, for the listening researcher or language teacher it is vital to establish the source of listening problems: ‘Until the teacher is provided with some sort of method of investigating the student’s problems, the teacher is really not in a position of being able to help the student “do better”’ (Brown, 1986: 286). Investigations of the routes by which listeners achieve understanding have adopted three main methods: ‘observation’, ‘introspection’ and ‘retrospection’. Observation Observation takes many forms, from informal noticing of real-life examples of misunderstandings (Bond and Garnes, 1980) to experiments designed to create ambiguities and referential conflicts (Brown, 1986). Numerous studies have investigated communication on map-based tasks (Brown et al., 1984; Brown, 1986; Yule and Powers, 1994). By adjusting the degree of difficulty built in to the tasks at specific ‘trouble spots’, the researcher can adjust the amount of negotiation required to resolve the problems. However, even in the most controlled of experiments the researcher cannot be certain of the cause of the listener’s doubts, or the current state of the listener’s mental model. Introspection One way of supplementing the information available from observation is to use introspection (comments by the listener at the time of listening or immediately afterwards). This form of inquiry is also known as the ‘think-aloud protocol’. A Download 1.71 Mb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling