Chapter 1 the study of collocations
Table 21. Post-hoc comparisons for the dependent variable: S-Nodes per T-
Download 0.8 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
colloca
- Bu sahifa navigatsiya:
- Table 22. Means and standard deviations for the dependent variable: S- Nodes per T-Unit
- 4.1.9 Summary of the Results for the Language Proficiency Analyses
Table 21. Post-hoc comparisons for the dependent variable: S-Nodes per T-
Unit Comparison Mean Diff. Fisher PLSD Scheffe F-test Dunnett t Group 1 vs. Group 2 -2.462 .691* 24.614* 7.016 Group 1 vs. Group 3 -4.747 .698* 89.551* 13.383 242 Group 2 vs. Group 3 -2.285 .693* 21.085* 6.494 * Significant at .05 level Table 22. Means and standard deviations for the dependent variable: S- Nodes per T-Unit GROUP COUNT MEAN STD. DEV. Group 1 91 1.119 .144 Group 2 94 1.366 .229 Group 3 90 1.594 .312 4.1.9 Summary of the Results for the Language Proficiency Analyses The results of the analyses for the language proficiency measures show an overall significant main effect for the factor Group. No difference was found between the three groups in the holistic rating of the essays, even though the three groups represent three different levels of language proficiency. This could be due to the nature of the holistic rating since it takes into account not just the use of language, but also the structure of the essay, its organisation, the expression of ideas, the explanations and arguments provided by the writer, etc. Therefore the ratings based on the holistic scale may obscure differences among subjects that are attributable to language proficiency, which is of most interest to this study. However, this lack of significant differences between groups using this measure is counter-balanced by the fact that reliable differences were found using the other measures, and these are in line with the 243 claim that the different groups are composed of subjects at different levels of proficiency, and possibly different stages of development. As can be seen from Table 16, there is a significant drop in Lexical Density for subjects in Groups 2 and 3 compared with subjects in Group 1, and for subjects in Group 3 compared with subjects in Group 2. To this extent, increases in proficiency appeared to be related to increases in the number of grammatical words used in the essays (as the TLU analysis showed). As a result, lower-level students were grammatically less accurate in their essays (as the results from the TLU analysis show), and thus the omission of grammatical words (e.g. articles) contributed to a higher percentage score for Lexical Density. In Group 3 where the students are grammatically more accurate, as the TLU analysis showed, the lexical density is lower. These results are also consistent with the findings of recent research which showed that subjects of lower proficiency levels use more content words, while those of higher proficiency levels use more function words , e.g. pronouns, articles, and prepositions (Ghadessy 1989). The results for the dependent variable Words per T-Unit also reflect different proficiency groupings (see Table 18). The higher the level of proficiency, the more subordination and embedding the student uses in the construction of sentences, and thus the longer the sentences they produce. This finding, it must be noted, is in partial agreement with the finding reported by Larsen-Freeman and Strom (1977), who found that the mean length of the T- Units in the writings of the subjects in their study increased steadily with 244 proficiency level, but the statistical analysis performed on their data did not yield significant differences. Larsen-Freeman and Strom conclude that length of T-Units is still "a viable contender on which to base an index of development" (Larsen-Freeman & Strom 1977:132). The results for the dependent variable Error-Free T-Units do show significant differences between the three groups (see Table 19). Although these differences support the claim that the three different groups reflect different proficiency groupings, the direction of the difference is in contrast to the findings of previous research. In line with Larsen-Freeman's findings (1978) it was expected that more proficient subjects would use more Error-Free T-Units than less proficient subjects. However, the present findings show that subjects in Group 1 use significantly more Error-Free T-Units than subjects in Group 2, and these subjects in turn use significantly more Error-Free T-Units than subjects in Group 3. The present results could be due to the fact that subjects in this study are simply trying harder to produce more complex syntax than less proficient subjects. It is certainly true that subjects in the present study are not at a sufficiently advanced level to make no mistakes in their writing, since the subjects in Group 3, who have had the longest period of instruction in English, and who are older by one and two years on average than subjects in the other groups, are only at a post-intermediate level. In Larsen-Freeman's study subjects were from a larger range of proficiency levels (5 groups), from subjects that were of very low proficiency and needed a great deal of ESL instruction (Group 1) to subjects that were advanced enough not to need any more ESL 245 instruction (Group 5). Even though Larsen-Freeman does not report the post- hoc comparisons for the Error-Free T-Units measure, it is apparent from the percentages reported in her paper that it is at the advanced level that subjects singificantly use more Error-Free T-Units, e.g. there is a 15% increase in the amount of Error-Free T-Units used by the advanced learners in group 5 (see Table 23). Download 0.8 Mb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling