Chapter 1 the study of collocations
particular community, or if it is an "arbitrary choice, in terms of linguistic
Download 0.8 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
colloca
particular community, or if it is an "arbitrary choice, in terms of linguistic structure, for the role of standard expression". For example, 'it's twenty to six' is a standard expression in English since it is a convention that one tells 'to [Hour]' rather than 'preceding [Hour]' or 'before [Hour]', and 'I want to marry you' is an arbitrarily established standard usage, compared to a less standard paraphrase such as 'I wish to be wedded to you', which could be used in a formal letter or a satirical speech (Pawley & Syder 1983:198). As with most of the theories examined so far, Pawley and Syder do not define the notion of lexicalised sentence stems any further, and they did not offer an explicit list of sentence stems that could be used as a framework in the study of collocations. The view that language consists of blocks or 'chunks' was also supported by Nattinger and DeCarrico (1992), who proposed the compilation of a lexical phrase dictionary for L2 learners. Nattinger and DeCarrico give the following examples of lexical phrases for inclusion in the dictionary: Conversational Maintenance (regularities of conversational interaction that describe how conversations begin, continue and end). Summoning: 153 Excuse/pardon me (sustained intonation); Hey/hi/hello, (Name); How are you (doing)? I didn't catch/get your name; Do you live around here? Hello, I'm + NAME; Good morning/afternoon/evening, (how are you) What's up? (Nattinger & DeCarrico 1993). From the examples of lexical phrases, as these were presented by Nattinger and DeCarrico, it appears that lexical phrases are not the same as collocations or lexicalised sentence stems. Lexical phrases appear to be more general than collocations and less systematic than lexicalised sentence stems. Also, Nattinger and DeCarrico are not concerned with providing explanations about why certain lexical phrases are put together, which would be more useful for the study of collocations. A set of criteria for examining whether a combination of words is a collocation or not is outlined by Kjellmer (1984), who also suggests the study of collocations in a grammatical framework. Kjellmer defines collocations as "lexically determined and grammatically restricted sequences of words" (Kjellmer 1984:163). According to this definition, only recurring sequences that are grammatically well-formed can be considered as collocations. For example, during a search of the Brown Corpus, Kjellmer found the following sequences: 'green ideas', 'try to', 'hall to'. From these strings, it is only 'hall to' and 'try to' that recur, and from these two, only 'try to' that is grammatically well-formed. Therefore, only 'try to' is a collocation (Kjellmer 1984:163). Kjellmer also tries to establish a set of rules for assessing 'collocational distinctiveness'. According to these, a sequence is highly distinctive when it appears frequently in many and 154 different categories of texts; it is long (minimum length is two words); and it is structurally complex. On the other hand, Renouf and Sinclair (1991) applied their theory of studying collocations to 'frameworks' consisting of discontinuous sequences of two words, whose grammatical well-formedness depends on what intervenes, e.g. 'a + ? + of', 'too + ? + to' (Renouf & Sinclair 1991:128). They found out that in some cases there seems to be a stronger collocational pull exerted by one of the pair on some items rather than on others, e.g. in the framework 'too + ? +to', 'to' would be able to collocate with 'easy', 'hard', 'good' and 'proud' even in the absence of 'too', e.g. ‘easy to do’, ‘good to do’, but not with 'much' or 'tired' which require the presence of 'too', e.g. ‘too tired to dance’, ‘too full to eat’, (Renouf & Sinclair 1991:133). Thus, Renouf and Sinclair demonstrated that the collocations of grammatical words offer an appropriate basis for studying collocations, since "co-occurrences in the language most commonly occur among grammatical words" (Renouf & Sinclair 1991:128). The importance of grammatical words for the study of collocations was also confirmed by Jones and Sinclair (1974). Even though their study on English lexical collocations was based on a relatively small corpus (147,000 running words), it yielded some interesting results concerning the study of collocation: the influence of the node does not extend beyond span position Node (N) + 4 (see also Berry-Rogghe 1973). Grammatical words are not collocationally neutral (unlike Haskel 1971). Even though grammatical words are weak at predicting their environment, they do show ability to predict word 155 classes at specific span positions, e.g. the collocates of the word 'the' in position N-1 are mainly verbs and prepositions, while in position N+1 they are nouns and adjectives. The significance of a collocation takes into account the overall frequency of the two items concerned, the number of times they occur together, and the length of the text. Collocations can appear to be 'text dependent'. Verbs tend to collocate with grammatical items, e.g. 'put' and 'take' collocate with a great number of prepositions to form phrasal verbs. Association between lexical items is subject to grammatical influence, e.g. the adjective 'good' is preceded by adverbs and followed by nouns as significant collocates. Significant collocations show a considerable amount of position dependence, e.g. in a span of 4, significant collocations most frequently occur in the span positions immediately next to the node, N-1 and N+1, while very little occurs at the two extremes of the span, N-4 and N+4. Finally, collocation was found to be an organising principle that influences the construction and interpretation of utterances (Jones & Sinclair 1974:48; Leitner 1992). The study of collocations in structural patterns was also suggested by Aisenstadt (1979). Aisenstadt distinguishes collocability restrictions as part of the wide field of collocability. Word combinations whose constituents are restricted in their 'commutability', i.e. their ability to combine with other words, are called restricted collocations (Aisenstadt 1979:71). Restricted collocations are defined as combinations of two or more words used in one of their regular, non-idiomatic meanings, following certain structural patterns (e.g. V+(art)+(A)+N), and restricted in their commutability not only by grammatical 156 and semantic valency (e.g. in the restricted collocation 'shrug one's shoulders' both components have a narrow semantic valency), but also by usage (e.g. we can 'bear a grudge' but we cannot *'bear hatred/ scorn') (Aisenstadt 1979:71, 1981:54). Restricted collocations are different from free word-combinations. For example 'carry' can enter a large number of free word-combinations when it means 'to support the weight of something' like 'carry a book/bag/chair/torch/table/etc.', but it may also enter a restricted collocation pattern 'carry conviction', 'carry persuasion', 'carry weight' when it is used to denote 'being convincing' or 'winning the argument' (Aisenstadt 1979:72). Some of the structural patterns of restricted collocations in English listed by Aisenstadt are given below in Table 2: Download 0.8 Mb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling