Common european framework of reference for languages: learning, teaching, assessment
Communicative language competences
Download 1.11 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
Framework EN.pdf(1)
- Bu sahifa navigatsiya:
- 5.2.1.1 lexical competence; 5.2.1.2 grammatical competence; 5.2.1.3 semantic competence; 5.2.1.4
- GENERAL LINGUISTIC RANGE C2
- VOCABULARY RANGE C2
- VOCABULARY CONTROL C2 Consistently correct and appropriate use of vocabulary. C1
- GRAMMATICAL ACCURACY C2
- PHONOLOGICAL CONTROL C2 As C1 C1
5.2 Communicative language competences For the realisation of communicative intentions, users/learners bring to bear their general capacities as detailed above together with a more specifically language-related communicative competence. Communicative competence in this narrower sense has the following components: • linguistic competences; • sociolinguistic competences; • pragmatic competences. 5.2.1 Linguistic competences No complete, exhaustive description of any language as a formal system for the expres- sion of meaning has ever been produced. Language systems are of great complexity and Users of the Framework may wish to consider and where appropriate state: • what study skills learners are encouraged/enabled to use and develop; • what heuristic abilities learners are encouraged/enabled to use and develop; • what provision is made for learners to become increasingly independent in their learning and use of language. Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: learning, teaching, assessment 108 the language of a large, diversified, advanced society is never completely mastered by any of its users. Nor could it be, since every language is in continuous evolution in response to the exigencies of its use in communication. Most nation states have attempted to establish a standard form of the language, though never in exhaustive detail. For its presentation, the model of linguistic description in use for teaching the corpus is still the same model as was employed for the long-dead classical languages. This ‘traditional’ model was, however, repudiated over 100 years ago by most profes- sional linguists, who insisted that languages should be described as they exist in use rather than as some authority thinks they should be and that the traditional model, having been developed for languages of a particular type, was inappropriate for the description of language systems with a very different organisation. However, none of the many proposals for alternative models has gained general acceptance. Indeed, the pos- sibility of one universal model of description for all languages has been denied. Recent work on linguistic universals has not as yet produced results which can be used directly to facilitate language learning, teaching and assessment. Most descriptive linguists are now content to codify practice, relating form and meaning, using terminology which diverges from traditional practice only where it is necessary to deal with phenomena outside the range of traditional models of description. This is the approach adopted in Section 4.2. It attempts to identify and classify the main components of linguistic com- petence defined as knowledge of, and ability to use, the formal resources from which well-formed, meaningful messages may be assembled and formulated. The scheme that follows aims only to offer as classificatory tools some parameters and categories which may be found useful for the description of linguistic content and as a basis for reflec- tion. Those practitioners who prefer to use a different frame of reference are free, here as elsewhere, to do so. They should then identify the theory, tradition or practice they are following. Here, we distinguish: 5.2.1.1 lexical competence; 5.2.1.2 grammatical competence; 5.2.1.3 semantic competence; 5.2.1.4 phonological competence; 5.2.1.5 Orthographic competence; 5.2.1.6 Orthoepic competence. Progress in the development of a learner’s ability to use linguistic resources can be scaled and is presented in that form below as appropriate. The user/learner’s competences 109 GENERAL LINGUISTIC RANGE C2 Can exploit a comprehensive and reliable mastery of a very wide range of language to formulate thoughts precisely, give emphasis, differentiate and eliminate ambiguity . . . No signs of having to restrict what he/she wants to say. C1 Can select an appropriate formulation from a broad range of language to express him/herself clearly, without having to restrict what he/she wants to say. Can express him/herself clearly and without much sign of having to restrict what he/she wants to say. B2 Has a sufficient range of language to be able to give clear descriptions, express viewpoints and develop arguments without much conspicuous searching for words, using some complex sentence forms to do so. Has a sufficient range of language to describe unpredictable situations, explain the main points in an idea or problem with reasonable precision and express thoughts on abstract or cultural topics such as music and films. B1 Has enough language to get by, with sufficient vocabulary to express him/herself with some hesitation and circumlocutions on topics such as family, hobbies and interests, work, travel, and current events, but lexical limitations cause repetition and even difficulty with formulation at times. Has a repertoire of basic language which enables him/her to deal with everyday situations with predictable content, though he/she will generally have to compromise the message and search for words. A2 Can produce brief everyday expressions in order to satisfy simple needs of a concrete type: personal details, daily routines, wants and needs, requests for information. Can use basic sentence patterns and communicate with memorised phrases, groups of a few words and formulae about themselves and other people, what they do, places, possessions etc. Has a limited repertoire of short memorised phrases covering predictable survival situations; frequent breakdowns and misunderstandings occur in non-routine situations. A1 Has a very basic range of simple expressions about personal details and needs of a concrete type. 5.2.1.1 Lexical competence, knowledge of, and ability to use, the vocabulary of a lan- guage, consists of lexical elements and grammatical elements. Lexical elements include: a) Fixed expressions, consisting of several words, which are used and learnt as wholes. Fixed expressions include: • sentential formulae, including: direct exponents of language functions (see section 5.2.3.2) such as greetings, e.g. How do you do? Good morning! etc. proverbs, etc. (see section 5.2.2.3) relict archaisms, e.g. Be off with you! • phrasal idioms, often: semantically opaque, frozen metaphors, e.g.: He kicked the bucket (i.e. he died). It’s a long shot (= unlikely to succeed). Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: learning, teaching, assessment 110 He drove hell for leather (i.e. very fast). intensifiers. Their use is often contextually and stylistically restricted, e.g. as white as snow (= ‘pure’), as against as white as a sheet (= ‘pallid’). • fixed frames, learnt and used as unanalysed wholes, into which words or phrases are inserted to form meaningful sentences, e.g.: ‘Please may I have . . .’. • other fixed phrases, such as: phrasal verbs, e.g. to put up with, to make do (with); compound prepositions, e.g. in front of. • fixed collocations, consisting of words regularly used together, e.g. to make a speech/mistake. b) Single word forms. A particular single word form may have several distinct meanings (polysemy), e.g. tank, a liquid container or an armoured armed vehicle. Single word forms include members of the open word classes: noun, verb, adjective, adverb, though these may include closed lexical sets (e.g. days of the week, months of the year, weights and measures, etc.). Other lexical sets may also be established for gram- matical and semantic purposes (see below). Grammatical elements belong to closed word classes, e.g. (in English): articles (a, the) quantifiers (some, all, many, etc.) demonstratives (this, that, these, those) personal pronouns (I, we, he, she, it, they, me, you, etc.) question words and relatives (who, what, which, where, how, etc.) possessives (my, your, his, her, its, etc.) prepositions (in, at, by, with, of, etc.) auxiliary verbs (be, do, have, modals) conjunctions (and, but, if, although) particles (e.g. in German: ja, wohl, aber, doch, etc.) Illustrative scales are available for the range of vocabulary knowledge, and the ability to control that knowledge. The user/learner’s competences 111 VOCABULARY RANGE C2 Has a good command of a very broad lexical repertoire including idiomatic expressions and colloquialisms; shows awareness of connotative levels of meaning. C1 Has a good command of a broad lexical repertoire allowing gaps to be readily overcome with circumlocutions; little obvious searching for expressions or avoidance strategies. Good command of idiomatic expressions and colloquialisms. B2 Has a good range of vocabulary for matters connected to his/her field and most general topics. Can vary formulation to avoid frequent repetition, but lexical gaps can still cause hesitation and circumlocution. B1 Has a sufficient vocabulary to express him/herself with some circumlocutions on most topics pertinent to his/her everyday life such as family, hobbies and interests, work, travel, and current events. Has sufficient vocabulary to conduct routine, everyday transactions involving familiar situations and topics. A2 Has a sufficient vocabulary for the expression of basic communicative needs. Has a sufficient vocabulary for coping with simple survival needs. A1 Has a basic vocabulary repertoire of isolated words and phrases related to particular concrete situations. VOCABULARY CONTROL C2 Consistently correct and appropriate use of vocabulary. C1 Occasional minor slips, but no significant vocabulary errors. B2 Lexical accuracy is generally high, though some confusion and incorrect word choice does occur without hindering communication. B1 Shows good control of elementary vocabulary but major errors still occur when expressing more complex thoughts or handling unfamiliar topics and situations. A2 Can control a narrow repertoire dealing with concrete everyday needs. A1 No descriptor available 5.2.1.2 Grammatical competence Grammatical competence may be defined as knowledge of, and ability to use, the gramm- atical resources of a language. Formally, the grammar of a language may be seen as the set of principles governing Users of the Framework may wish to consider and where appropriate state: • which lexical elements (fixed expressions and single word forms) the learner will need/be equipped/be required to recognise and/or use; • how they are selected and ordered. Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: learning, teaching, assessment 112 the assembly of elements into meaningful labelled and bracketed strings (sentences). Grammatical competence is the ability to understand and express meaning by produc- ing and recognising well-formed phrases and sentences in accordance with these princi- ples (as opposed to memorising and reproducing them as fixed formulae). The grammar of any language in this sense is highly complex and so far defies definitive or exhaustive treatment. There are a number of competing theories and models for the organisation of words into sentences. It is not the function of the Framework to judge between them or to advocate the use of any one, but rather to encourage users to state which they have chosen to follow and what consequences their choice has for their practice. Here we limit ourselves to identifying some parameters and categories which have been widely used in grammatical description. The description of grammatical organisation involves the specification of: • elements, e.g.: morphs morphemes-roots and affixes words • categories, e.g.: number, case, gender concrete/abstract, countable/uncountable (in)transitive, active/passive voice past/present/future tense progressive, (im)perfect aspect • classes, e.g.: conjugations declensions open word classes: nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs, closed word classes (grammatical elements – see section 5.2.1.1) • structures, e.g.: compound and complex words phrases: (noun phrase, verb phrase, etc.) clauses: (main, subordinate, co-ordinate) sentences: (simple, compound, complex) • processes (descriptive), e.g.: nominalisation affixation suppletion gradation transposition transformation • relations, e.g.: government concord valency An illustrative scale is available for grammatical accuracy. This scale should be seen in relation to the scale for general linguistic range shown at the beginning of this section. It is not considered possible to produce a scale for progression in respect of grammatical structure which would be applicable across all languages. The user/learner’s competences 113 GRAMMATICAL ACCURACY C2 Maintains consistent grammatical control of complex language, even while attention is otherwise engaged (e.g. in forward planning, in monitoring others’ reactions). C1 Consistently maintains a high degree of grammatical accuracy; errors are rare and difficult to spot. Good grammatical control; occasional ‘slips’ or non-systematic errors and minor flaws in sentence structure may still occur, but they are rare and can often be corrected in retrospect. B2 Shows a relatively high degree of grammatical control. Does not make mistakes which lead to misunderstanding. Communicates with reasonable accuracy in familiar contexts; generally good control though with noticeable mother tongue influence. Errors occur, but it is clear what he/she is trying to express. B1 Uses reasonably accurately a repertoire of frequently used ‘routines’ and patterns associated with more predictable situations. A2 Uses some simple structures correctly, but still systematically makes basic mistakes – for example tends to mix up tenses and forget to mark agreement; nevertheless, it is usually clear what he/she is trying to say. A1 Shows only limited control of a few simple grammatical structures and sentence patterns in a learnt repertoire. A distinction is traditionally drawn between morphology and syntax. Morphology deals with the internal organisation of words. Words may be analysed into morphemes, classed as: • roots, or stems; • affixes (prefixes, suffixes, infixes), including: word-forming affixes (e.g. re-, un-, -ly, -ness); inflexional affixes (e.g. s, -ed, -ing). Word-formation: Words may be classified into: • simple words (root only, e.g. six, tree, break); • complex words (root + affixes, e.g. unbrokenly, sixes); • compound words (containing more than one root, e.g. sixpence, breakdown, oak-tree, evening dress). Users of the Framework may wish to consider and where appropriate state: • on which theory of grammar they have based their work; • which grammatical elements, categories, classes, structures, processes and relations are learners, etc. equipped/required to handle. Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: learning, teaching, assessment 114 Morphology also deals with other ways of modifying word forms, e.g.: • vowel alteration (sing/sang/sung, mouse/mice) • consonant modification (lend/lent) • irregular forms (bring/brought, catch/caught) • suppletion (go/went) • zero forms (sheep/sheep, cut/cut/cut) Morphophonology deals with the phonetically conditioned variation of morphemes (e.g. English s/z/iz in walks, lies, rises; t/d/id in laughed, cried, shouted), and their morph- ologically conditioned phonetic variation (e.g. i:/e in creep/crept, mean/meant, weep/ wept). Syntax deals with the organisation of words into sentences in terms of the categories, elements, classes, structures, processes and relations involved, often presented in the form of a set of rules. The syntax of the language of a mature native speaker is highly complex and largely unconscious. The ability to organise sentences to convey meaning is a central aspect of communicative competence. 5.2.1.3 Semantic competence deals with the learner’s awareness and control of the organisation of meaning. Lexical semantics deals with questions of word meaning, e.g.: • relation of word to general context: reference; connotation; exponence of general specific notions; • interlexical relations, such as: synonymy/antonymy; hyponymy; collocation; part-whole relations; componential analysis; translation equivalence. Users of the Framework may wish to consider and where appropriate state: • what grammatical elements, categories, classes, structures, processes and relations learners will need/be equipped/required to handle. Users of the Framework may wish to consider and where appropriate state: • what morphological elements and processes the learner will need/be equipped/required to handle. The user/learner’s competences 115 Grammatical semantics deals with the meaning of grammatical elements, categories, structures and processes (see section 5.2.1.2). Pragmatic semantics deals with logical relations such as entailment, presupposition, implicature, etc. Questions of meaning are of course central to communication and are treated passim in this Framework (see particularly section 5.1.1.1). Linguistic competence is treated here in a formal sense. From the point of view of theoretical or descriptive linguistics, a language is a highly complex symbolic system. When an attempt is made, as here, to separate out the many different components of com- municative competence, knowledge (largely unconscious) of and ability to handle formal structure is legitimately identifiable as one of those components. How much, if indeed any, of this formal analysis should enter into language learning or teaching is a different matter. The functional/notional approach adopted in the Council of Europe publications Waystage 1990, Threshold Level 1990 and Vantage Level offers an alternative to the treatment of linguistic competence in Section 5.2.1–3. Instead of starting from language forms and their meanings, it starts from a systematic classification of communicative functions and of notions, divided into general and specific, and secondarily deals with forms, lexical and grammatical, as their exponents. The approaches are complementary ways of dealing with the ‘double articulation’ of language. Languages are based on an organisation of form and an organisation of meaning. The two kinds of organisation cut across each other in a largely arbitrary fashion. A description based on the organisation of the forms of expression atomises meaning, and that based on the organisation of meaning atomises form. Which is to be preferred by the user will depend on the purpose for which the description is produced. The success of the Threshold Level approach indicates that many practitioners find it more advantageous to go from meaning to form rather than the more traditional practice of organising progression in purely formal terms. On the other hand, some may prefer to use a ‘communicative grammar’, as for example, in Un niveau-seuil. What is clear is that a language learner has to acquire both forms and meanings. 5.2.1.4 Phonological competence involves a knowledge of, and skill in the perception and production of: • the sound-units (phonemes) of the language and their realisation in particular con- texts (allophones); • the phonetic features which distinguish phonemes (distinctive features, e.g. voicing, rounding, nasality, plosion); • the phonetic composition of words (syllable structure, the sequence of phonemes, word stress, word tones); • sentence phonetics (prosody) • sentence stress and rhythm • intonation; Users of the Framework may wish to consider and where appropriate state: • what kinds of semantic relation learners are equipped/required to build up/demonstrate. Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: learning, teaching, assessment 116 • phonetic reduction • vowel reduction • strong and weak forms • assimilation • elision. PHONOLOGICAL CONTROL C2 As C1 C1 Can vary intonation and place sentence stress correctly in order to express finer shades of meaning. Download 1.11 Mb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling