穨Review. Pdf
Download 453.46 Kb. Pdf ko'rish
|
Thesis Liang Tsailing
- Bu sahifa navigatsiya:
- 5.1 Effects of Cooperative Learning and Language Learning
CHAPTER FIVE
DISCUSSIONS, CONCLUSIONS, AND IMPLICATIONS The results presented in Chapter Four suggest that the students studying in the cooperative context outperform the students in the control group who study English in the traditional method. The effects of cooperative learning seem salient in enhancing the EFL junior high school students’ language learning, especially their communicative competence, and motivation toward learning English as a foreign language. The high- and low-achievers are able to grow at their own pace, and, at the same time, contribute to their peers’ learning. The results yielded in this study will be discussed according to the research questions: 1. What are the effects of cooperative learning on the improvement of the EFL learners’ language learning in terms of communicative competence and the school monthly achievement tests? 2. What are the effects of cooperative learning on the EFL learners’ motivation toward learning English as a foreign language? 3. What are the effects of cooperative learning on the high/low achievers in a heterogeneous class? Based upon the findings discussed, guidelines of implementing cooperative learning are thus proposed and conclusions are drawn. The pedagogical implications, limitations of the present study, and suggestions for further research are also included in this chapter. 124 5.1 Effects of Cooperative Learning and Language Learning The significant gains of the experimental group on the interaction-based oral task supported Brown’s (1994) and Kagan’s (1995) views that cooperative learning was actually a practice that could put the communicative approach into action. Due to the socially oriented lessons taught and learned through small group interaction, the students in the experimental group were able to demonstrate better, and significantly better, linguistic competence, discourse competence, strategic competence, and non-verbal communicative competence than the control group. Such findings were congruent with Wei’s (1997) claim that cooperative learning was considered the must and the best instructional format enhancing learner’s communicative competence. The possible reasons to account for the significant gains in the experimental group in terms of their improvement in the four aspects of the oral communicative competence could be synthesized into the following categories: (1) the increase of student talk through comprehensible input, interaction, and output; (2) the incentive structures of positive reinforcement; and (3) the supportive and communicative learning context. These three mechanisms of cooperative learning seemed to contribute to the students’ oral communicative competence, as demonstrated in the results of this study. In a cooperative learning context, there were many interactive tasks that would naturally stimulate the students' cognitive, linguistic, and social abilities. Cooperative activities tended to integrate the acquisition of these skills and create powerful learning opportunities. As Wei (1997) stated, interactions between more than two persons were the necessities for effective communication activities and oral practice. The experimental group was endowed with more opportunities to actually 125 practice the target language in class through many of the student-centered activities. Almost in each lesson, the students were asked to practice and perform the dialogues in the textbook with their talk pairs who sat in front of them until they could talk freely without referring back to their textbooks. The frequent practice of the dialogues with talk-pairs and the Inside-Outside Circle might be an important factor contributing to the students’ acquisition of oral communicative competence. And both the self-correction and the peer-correction occurred during the student-centered activities also contributed to encourage the active roles of the students. The inter- and intra-group significant gains of the oral tasks in the experimental group, as shown in Table 4.3 and Table 4.4, corresponded to the three arguments suggested by Liang (1996): (1) group work helped students to overcome the anxiety of speaking a foreign language because speaking with a peer is less threatening than speaking to a teacher in front of the whole class; (2) group activities gave students more opportunities to use the target language; (3) working in a group could largely reduce the anxiety of speaking a foreign language in class. Thus, the quality of communication of the experimental group was better than that in the control group. And the amount of student-talk was further maximized by activities that involved pair work and group work, which engaged all the students in speaking. Almost up to 80 percent of the class time in the experimental group was scheduled for activities that included a lot of student talk in the target language. And the student talk was done simultaneously so that almost all of the students were engaged in language production and practice. The student-centered method of cooperative learning helped to increase the active communication for the students in the experimental group. The increase of student talk in the experimental group indicated that cooperative learning could foster language development through increased active communication 126 and frequent use of the target language for academic and social purposes (Cohen, 1984; Dörnyei & Malderez, 1997; Kagan, 1995). According to Wei (1997), many of the activities in a cooperative learning language class corresponded to those advocated in the communicative approach. Ghaith and Shaaban (1995) also argued that cooperative learning used in language teaching often “result in higher quality of discourse competence as the students better comprehend each other as well as take opportunities to practice their paralinguistic skills—gestures, facial, and shoulder expressions, and so on (p. 26).” In other words, in a less threatening learning context as that of cooperative learning, the students in the experimental group were able to demonstrate higher oral classroom participation, which was related to their statistical significant gain in the language proficiency (Lin, 1993; Zhou, 1991; Zhou, 2002), and higher level of peer interaction, which was an essential feature of learning when the learners were in the action of interacting with people in their environment and in cooperation with their peers (Vygotsky, 1978). Such findings of the significant improvement of the students’ oral communicative competence, as shown in Table 4.3, were similar to Bejarano’s (1987) field experiment of the ESL junior high school learners in Israel who studied in cooperative learning performed better on overall English proficiency than the control group. In addition to the learning of the verbal communicative competence, some non-verbal features of such competence also developed along with the increased amount of student talk. The non-verbal features could reduce or enhance the effects of verbal communication (Upshur, 1979). The non-verbal features of eye contact, smile, and proper conversational distance identified in this study corresponded to Upshur (1979)’s discussion of non-linguistic factors on second language performance tests. As Upshur (1979) observed, sometimes the non-verbal factors can affect the 127 results of verbal expressions, as discussed in the literature review in Chapter Two. The experimental group had more opportunities to formally and informally interact with their teacher as well as their peers, and, therefore, had more chances to be corrected by their teacher whenever inappropriate behaviors occurred. With the frequent encounter of new talk pairs through the Inside-Outside Circle, it forced the students in the experimental group to use more facial expressions, hand gestures, or even body language to make them understood than those in the control group. Being able to display eye contact and smile might be attributed to the reason that the students felt more secured and supportive learning with their peers. Role-playing might not seem as threatening as they experienced in the previous semester when they were isolated learners. The smile might also be an indicator that they started to enjoy English class or they felt more relaxed in the cooperative context. Maybe that was the reason why the experimental group displayed more eye contact and smile during the oral task. Smile could be an essential non-verbal language in communication, especially when some of the students forgot their lines during the oral task. Their partners’ smile would be very encouraging and supportive at this critical moment. It was interesting to note that when the students were able to express verbal apology to their partners, they were able to smile. Smile was the natural body language to accompany that verbal apology. Though unable to utter verbal apology, at least one student from the experimental group still managed to smile to reduce the awkwardness and embarrassment caused by his own silence. It seemed that most students in the experimental group were not totally frightened by the occurrence of communication breakdown. They were, at least, able to keep eye contact with their Download 453.46 Kb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling