Book · January 994 citations 110 reads 2,264 authors
Download 5.72 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
1994 Book DidacticsOfMathematicsAsAScien
All together, what is it?
Child: It is paper. Teacher: What is it? not what is it made of? What's its name? Child: It's written? Teacher: No. Child: . . . (silence) Teacher: You have said that it has eyes, a nose, and so on. What is it? Child: . . . (silence) MARIA G. BARTOLINI BUSSI This episode is taken from the observation protocol of a one-to-one interac- tion between an elementary school teacher (Bondesan, personal communication) and a low achiever (1st grade): The child already knows the teacher and the climate is very relaxed. This special interaction (a remedial workshop) was designed for low achievers in order to foster the development of planning and designing strategies by means of verbal language as a prerequisite for mathematical problem-solving (Boero, 1992). The goal of this session is to build a copy of the puppet while verbalizing the process. The child is a 1st grader with learning disabilities; she is not handicapped, but she has lacked family experiences of joint activity in which action is systematically accompanied by speech. As the protocol shows, she can name the different parts of the object, but cannot name the whole. The teacher feels responsible for unblocking the child, because of institutional needs (the very purpose of that remedial workshop) and for personal needs (the "revolutionary" will to offer equal opportunities to every child). What has theory to offer her? Two radical competing positions are offered by Piagetian versus Vygotskyan researchers: act as a clinical interviewer, encourage the child to express herself and to build her own knowledge; act as a guide, help the child, lend her the right gestures and words. Actually, the teacher behaved as a Vygotskyan and successfully offered the child actions and utterances to be imitated; maybe, being Piagetian, in this radical sense, could have resulted in abandoning the child to her destiny. 4.2 When Mathematical Behavior is Against Everyday Behavior The problem of mathematical proof seems to be one of the crucial issues of didactics where advanced thinking is concerned. Balacheff (1990b) studied the students' treatment of a refutation by means of social interaction. His work confirmed the usefulness of social interaction, but enlightened its lim- its too, because of the major role played by argumentation. In a specific study on deductive thinking, Duval (1991) showed that the rules of deduc- tive reasoning are very different from the rules of argumentative reasoning. The strategy that the same author experimented successfully to make the students (aged 13-14) distinguish between argumentative and deductive rea- soning is supposed to be more Vygotskyan than Piagetian (actually, in the paper, disagreement with Piaget is explicitly stated even if Vygotsky is not referred to): They were given the rules for building an oriented proposi- tional graph, to connect hypotheses to conclusions (a good example of semiotic mediation). We could even be critical about such an introduction of rules to be followed if they are perceived by students as rules of classroom contract only. Yet what seems to me unquestionable is that deductive rea- soning depends on social factors: When students are approaching Teacher: Is it a child? Child: . . . (silence) 129 130 mathematical proof, they are entering a flow of thought that was (and still is) developed outside school by mathematicians, together with a related system of values as well as of acceptable behaviors. To cope with this problem, it is not sufficient to consider mathematics as an individual subjective construction, it is necessary to consider mathematics as a Download 5.72 Mb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling