Handbook of psychology volume 7 educational psychology


Download 9.82 Mb.
Pdf ko'rish
bet80/153
Sana16.07.2017
Hajmi9.82 Mb.
#11404
1   ...   76   77   78   79   80   81   82   83   ...   153

References

331

Stark, R. E., Rose, S. N., & McClagen, A. (1975). Features of infant

sounds: The first eight weeks of life. Journal of Child Language,

2, 205–222.

Strain, P. S., & Hoyson, M. (2000). The need for longitudinal, in-

tensive social skills intervention: LEAP follow-up outcomes for

children with autism. Topics in Early Childhood Special Educa-



tion, 20(2), 116 –122.

Sulzby, E. (1985). Children’s emergent reading of favourite story-

books: A developmental study. Reading Research Quarterly, 20,

427– 446.

Sulzby, E., & Teale, W. (1991). Emergent literacy. In R. Barr, M.

Kamil, P. Mosenthall, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of read-



ing research: Vol. 2 (pp. 727–757). New York: Longman.

Takhvar, M. (1988). Play and theories of play: A review of the liter-

ature. Early Child Development and Care, 39, 221–244.

Taylor, D. (1983). Family literacy. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.

Teale, W., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy as a perspective

for examining how young children become readers and writers.

In W. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy: Writing and

reading (pp. vii–xxv). Norwood, MA: Ablex.

Templeton, S., & Morris, D. (1999). Questions teachers ask about

spelling. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 102–112.

There is no Utopia: The story of returning immigrants. (1999,

December 23). The Hangyurea Newsletter, Issue No. 288. Re-

trieved from http://www.hani.co.kr/h21/data/l991213/1pascd 01



.html.

Vandell, D. L., &. Corasaniti, M. A. (1990). Variations in early child

care: Do they predict subsequent social, emotional, and cogni-

tive differences? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 5, 55–72.

Villarruel, F. A., Imig, D. R., & Kostelnik, M. J. (1995). Diverse

families. In E. Garcia & B. McLaughlin (Eds.), Meeting the



challenge of linguistic and cultural diversity in early childhood

education (pp. 103–124). New York: Teachers College Press.

Volk, D. (1997). Questions in lessons: Activity settings in the homes

and school of two Puerto Rican kindergarteners. Anthropology

and Education Quarterly, 28(1), 22– 49.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1976). Play and its role in the mental development

of the child. In J. Bruner, J. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its

role in development and evolution (pp. 537–554). New York:

Basic Books.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in a society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

University Press. 

Wang, S. (1987). Yani as I see her. In Wang Yani: Pictures by a

young Chinese girl (pp. 15–23). Munich: Prestel.

Weisner, T. S., Gallimore, R., & Jordan, C. (1993). Unpacking

cultural effects of classroom learning: Hawaiian peer assistance

and child-generated activity. In R. N. Roberts (Ed.), Coming



home to preschool: The sociocultural context of early education

(pp. 59–87). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Wells, G. (1985). Preschool literacy-related activities and success

in school. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrence, & A. Hildyard (Eds.),



Literacy language and learning: The nature and consequences

of reading and writing (pp. 229–255). Cambridge, England:

Cambridge University Press.

Whitebook, M., Howes, C., & Phillips, D. (1990). Who cares? Child

care teachers and the quality of care in America. Final Report,

National Child Care Staffing Study. Oakland, CA: Child Care

Employee Project.

Wilson, B., & Wilson, M. (1977). An iconoclastic view of the im-

agery sources in the drawings of young people. Art Education,

30(1), 5–11.

Wilson, B., & Wilson, M. (1981). Review of Artful scribbles: The



significance of children’s drawings, by H. Gardner. Studies in

Visual Communication, 7(1), 86 –89.

Winner, E. (1996). Gifted children: Myths and realities. New York:

Basic Books.

Winton, P. J. (2000). Early childhood intervention personnel prepa-

ration: Backward mapping for future planning. Topics in Early

Childhood Special Education, 20(2), 87–94.

Wolf, D. (1994). Development as growth of repertoires. In M. F.

Franklin & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Development and the arts

(pp. 59–78). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Wolf, D., & Perry, M. (1988). From endpoints to repertoires: New

conclusions about drawing development. Journal of Aesthetic



Education, 22(1), 17–35.

Wong Fillmore, L. (1991). When learning a second language

means losing the first? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 6,

323–346.


Wright, S. C., & Taylor, D. M. (1995). Identity and the language of

the classroom: Investing the impact of heritage versus second

language instruction on personal and collective self-esteem.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 241–252.

Wright, S. C., Taylor, D. M., & Macarthur, J. (2000). Subtractive

bilingualism and the survival of the Inuit language: Heritage-

versus second-language education. Journal of Educational



Psychology, 92(1), 63–84.

Yen, C. C., & Ispa, J. M. (2000). Children’s temperament and be-

havior in Montessori and constructivist early childhood programs.

Early Education and Development, 11(2), 171–186.

Zigler, E., & Styfco, S. J. (2000). Pioneering steps (and fumbles) in

developing a federal preschool intervention. Topics in Early

Childhood Special Education, 20(2), 67–70.

Zimmerman, M. P. (1986). Music development in middle child-

hood: A summary of selected research studies. Bulletin of the

Council for Research in Music Education, 86, 18–35.


CHAPTER 14

Psychology of Literacy and Literacy Instruction

MICHAEL PRESSLEY



333

EMERGENT LITERACY DURING

THE PRESCHOOL YEARS

334


Emergent Literacy

334

Phonemic Awareness

336

FIRST GRADE AND THE PRIMARY YEARS

337

Word Recognition

337

Teaching Primary-Level Students to

Sound Out Words

338

Reading Recovery

339

Studies of Exceptional Primary-Level Teachers

340

Summary

342

COMPREHENSION

343

Fluent Word Recognition

343

Vocabulary

343

Comprehension Strategies

343

Summary

344

WRITING


344

ENCOURAGING ADULT LITERACY

346

Basic, Word-Level Difficulties

346

Comprehension Difficulties

346

Writing Difficulties

347

Summary

348

CLOSING COMMENTS

348

REFERENCES



348

When first asked whether I could prepare a chapter summa-

rizing literacy research, my initial response was that the

request was impossible. What came to mind immediately

were the three volumes of the Handbook of Reading Re-

search (Barr, Kamil, Mosenthal, & Pearson, 1991; Kamil,

Mosenthal, Pearson, & Barr, 2000; Pearson, Barr, Kamil, &

Mosenthal, 1984), the most prominent compendiums of read-

ing research, which collectively include 3,000 pages to sum-

marize just reading research (although some writing research

found its way into those volumes). 

Even more daunting than just the volume of research, how-

ever, is its diversity. From a methodological perspective, there

are experimental and correlational traditions in literacy studies.

In recent years, however, such traditional and quantitative ap-

proaches have been supplanted largely by more qualitative

methods, including ethnographies (Florio-Ruane & McVee,

2000), verbal protocol analyses (Afflerbach, 2000; Pressley &

Afflerbach, 1995), narrative approaches (Alvermann, 2000),

and single-subject designs (Neuman & McCormick, 2000).

Conceptually, literacy at one time was primarily seen from

a behavioral perspective, with such behaviorism yielding to

cognitivism in the 1970s and 1980s. Although there is still

much cognitive study of reading, sociocultural emphasis in

the field has been increasing, beginning in the 1990s and mov-

ing into the twenty-first century (Gaffney & Anderson, 2000). 

Literacy is also a decidedly international field of study;

exciting ideas have come from Australia and New Zealand

(Wilkinson, Freebody, & Elkins, 2000), the United Kingdom

(Harrison, 2000), Latin America (Santana, 2000), and in-

creasingly from former Iron Curtain countries (Meredith &

Steele, 2000). Although much of literacy instruction has

been and remains focused on kindergarten through Grade 12

instruction, in recent decades a great deal of work has been

done on literacy development during the preschool years

(Yaden, Rowe, & McGillivray, 2000) as well as research ex-

tending into the college years (Flippo & Caverly, 2000) and

beyond (Kirsch, Jungeblut, Jenkins, & Kolstad, 1993). Also,

there has been a clear shift away from thinking about literacy

as a development that occurs purely in the schools; it is now

conceived as more an acquisition that occurs in families,

(Purcell-Gates, 2000) in the workplace, and in the larger, in-

creasingly technological community (Reinking, McKenna,

Labbo, & Kieffer, 1998). 

Of course, one way to deal with this enormous and multi-

dimensionally expanding literature would be to focus only on

the parts that are decidedly psychological because much of lit-

eracy research was not carried out by psychologists and seems

rather far afield from psychological issues; in fact, that is a

tactic taken in this chapter. The downside of this approach is

that some of the most interesting and cutting-edge directions



334

Psychology of Literacy and Literacy Instruction

are neglected. Some ideas that might start psychologists

thinking about new directions they might pursue are not put

before readers’ eyes. The serious scholar in literacy—or any-

one who wants to have a broadly informed opinion—will (at

a minimum) spend much time with the 3,000 Handbook pages

now available at the beginning of this millennium.

Another tactic that I employ here is to focus on primary and

significant issues and questions—ones that have been of con-

cern for a very long time. This approach in particular makes

sense because it does lead to some answers—that is, a number

of important issues in reading and writing have been studied

long enough that replicable findings have emerged. This em-

phasis on replicable findings—on the surface at least—makes

this chapter consistent with the approach of the National Read-

ing Panel (2000). I am inconsistent with the National Reading

Panel, however, in that I am willing to consider a greater di-

versity of methods than that group was. That group generally

limited itself to experimental studies; it admitted only the oc-

casional quasi-experimental study and distanced itself from

qualitative approaches entirely. This chapter certainly does

present much coverage of outcomes produced in true experi-

ments and approximations to experiments, but these out-

comes are complemented by other scientific findings as well.

In particular, descriptive methods, including ethnographies,

have provided rich understandings about the complexities of

some important instructional approaches—understandings

that never would be produced in true experiments or repre-

sented in the write-ups of experimental studies.

This chapter could have been organized in a number of dif-

ferent ways; I have decided to organize this one along devel-

opmental lines. In fact, there have been studies of literacy

development beginning in late infancy and proceeding through

adulthood. Of course, what develops varies with each develop-

mental period; the development of general language compe-

tencies is particularly critical during the preschool years.

Although beginning reading instruction during the early ele-

mentary school years focuses on the development of letter- and

word-level competencies in reading and writing, this focus

eventually gives way to the development of fluent reading as a

goal and increasing concerns with comprehension and compo-

sition in the later elementary and middle school grades. By

high school and college, much of the emphasis is on honing lit-

eracy skills in the service of the learning demands of secondary

and postsecondary education. Researchers interested in adult

literacy have often focused on adults who did not develop lit-

eracy competencies during the schooling years; such research

generally attempts to develop interventions to promote literacy

in these populations, whose members often suffer socio-

economic and personal disadvantages directly attributable to

their reading problems.

EMERGENT LITERACY DURING

THE PRESCHOOL YEARS

What happens to children during the preschool years relates

to later literacy development. Many developmentalists inter-

ested in literacy have focused on what is known as emergent



literacy, which is the development of the language skills un-

derlying literacy through interactions with the social world.

Other developmentalists who have been interested in chil-

dren’s beginning letter-level and word-recognition skills have

focused more on a competency known as phonemic aware-

ness, which is the awareness that words are composed of

sounds blended together.



Emergent Literacy

One of the more heavily researched topics by developmental

psychologists is the nature of mother-infant attachment.

When interactions between the principal caregiver and an in-

fant are constructive and caring, the attachment that develops

can be described as secure (Bowlby, 1969). In particular,

when parents are responsive to the child and provide for its

needs, secure attachment is more likely. The securely at-

tached baby interacts with the world comfortably in the care-

giver’s presence and responds favorably to the caregiver after

a period of caregiver absence.

Matas, Arend, and Sroufe (1978) made a fundamentally

important discovery. Children who experience secure at-

tachment during infancy engage in more effective problem

solving with their parents during the preschool years. When

parents are securely attached to their children, they are more

likely to provide appropriate degrees of support as their chil-

dren attempt to solve problems (Frankel & Bates, 1990;

Matas et al., 1978).

A related finding is that when parents and preschoolers are

securely attached, they interact more productively in situa-

tions involving literacy. Bus and van IJzendoorn (1988)

observed both securely attached and insecurely attached

mother-child pairs as they watched Sesame Street together,

read a picture book, and went through an alphabet book. The

interactions involving securely attached parents and children

were much more positive than were the interactions between

insecurely attached parents and children. Securely attached

preschoolers were more attentive and less easily distracted

during interactions, and much more literate activity was ob-

served in the interactions of securely attached pairs compared

to those of insecurely attached pairs. Storybook reading was

more intense with the secure pairs than with the insecure

pairs; the secure parent-child pairs talked more about the

story than did the insecure pairs. An especially interesting


Emergent Literacy During the Preschool Years

335

finding was that securely attached parents and their 3-year-

old children reported doing more reading together (Bus &

van IJzendoorn, 1995). 

That storybook reading brings greater rewards when at-

tachment security is greater is an important finding because

high-quality storybook reading during the preschool years

clearly promotes literacy development. There are clear corre-

lations between the amount of storybook reading during the

preschool years and subsequent language development, chil-

dren’s interest in reading, and their success as beginning

readers (Sulzby & Teale, 1991); this is sensible because

storybook reading at its best is a rich verbal experience, with

much questioning and answering by both reader and child.

Storybook reading permits practice at working out meaning

from words in text and pictures, as well as opportunities for

the child to practice relating ideas in stories to their own lives

and the world as they understand it (Applebee & Langer, 1983;

Cochran-Smith, 1984; Flood, 1977; Pelligrini, Perlmutter,

Galda, & Brody, 1990; Roser & Martinez, 1985; Taylor &

Strickland, 1986). As a child matures and gains experience

with storybook reading, the conversations between reader and

child increase in complexity (Snow, 1983; Sulzby & Teale,

1987). Older preschoolers who have had much storybook

reading experience are much more attentive during such read-

ing than are same-age peers who have had relatively little op-

portunity to experience books with their parents or other adults

(Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1988). Many correlational data sup-

port the hypothesis that storybook reading is beneficial for

children’s cognitive development—that it stimulates language

development and sets the stage for beginning reading.

This body of evidence in the context of storybook read-

ing is complemented by other data substantiating striking

connections between the richness of preschoolers’ verbal

worlds and subsequent language development. One of the

most ambitious and most cited analyses was made by Uni-

versity of Kansas psychologists Hart and Risley (1995).

They observed 42 families for 2.5 years, beginning in the

second semester of a child’s life. During these observations,

they recorded all actions and interactions. The first im-

portant finding was that there were significant differences

between families in both the quality and the extensiveness

of verbal interactions. The quality of interactions in terms of

completeness and complexity of language was greater in

professional homes than in working-class homes, and lan-

guage complexity in working-class homes was greater than

in welfare homes—that is, in homes of higher socioeco-

nomic status, parents listened more to their children, they

asked their children to elaborate their comments more, and

they taught their children how to cope verbally when con-

fronted with ideas that were challenging for the children to

communicate. Quantitatively, the differences in verbal inter-

actions were really striking: Whereas a child in a profes-

sional home might experience 4 million verbalizations a

year, a child in a welfare family could be exposed to only

250,000 utterances. Did these vast differences in experience

translate into later performance differences? There was no

doubt about it; superior language was detected by age 3 in

the children raised in professional families compared to

children in working-class and welfare families.

Of course, the problem with correlational data is that

causality is never clear. Yes, it could be that the richer experi-

ences promoted language development, or it could be that

more verbal children stimulated richer language interactions

during storybook reading and throughout their days. Fortu-

nately, complementary experimental studies establish more

definitively that high-quality verbal interactions result in

linguistic advances in children. 

Grover Whitehurst and his colleagues (Whitehurst et al.,

1988) hypothesized that if parents were coached in order to

improve their verbal interactions with their children during

storybook reading, the language functioning of the children

would improve. Whitehurst et al. worked for a month with

the parents of 14 children between the ages of 1.5–3 years. In

particular, the parents were taught to use more open-ended

questions as they read storybooks with their children; they

were also taught to ask more questions about the functions

and attributes of objects in stories. Whitehurst et al. (1988)

also taught the parents to elaborate and expand on comments

made by their children during reading. In short, the parents

were taught the tricks of the trade for stimulating productive

and verbally rich conversations with young children. In

contrast, parents and children in a control condition simply

continued to read together for the month corresponding to

treatment for the experimental participants.

First, the intervention worked in that it did increase the

verbal complexity and extensiveness of communications

between parents and children. Although experimental and

control parent-child interactions were similar before the

study, the experimental group conversations during book

reading were much richer following the intervention. More-

over, clear differences appeared in the language functioning

of the experimental group children following the interven-

tion, reflected by performance on standardized tests of psy-

cholinguistic ability and vocabulary. These effects have been

replicated several times, both by Whitehurst’s associates

(Valdez-Menchaca & Whitehurst, 1992; Whitehurst et al.,

1994) and by others (Crain-Thoresen & Dale, 1995; Dickinson

& Smith, 1994; Lonigan, Anthony, & Burgess, 1995).

In short, evidence suggests that preschool verbal experi-

ences promote language development, potentially in ways


Download 9.82 Mb.

Do'stlaringiz bilan baham:
1   ...   76   77   78   79   80   81   82   83   ...   153




Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling