Polysemy and metaphor in perception verbs: a cross-linguistic study
Download 1.39 Mb. Pdf ko'rish
|
PhD-Thesis-99
- Bu sahifa navigatsiya:
- 5.1.1.1. Vision
5.1.1. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SENSES
A complete understanding of how human perception works must include descriptions of the three elements (PR, OP, P) mentioned above. It is necessary to understand which parts of our body and brain are involved in the perceptual process, and how these organs work. The reason for this is that the function and limitations of these systems shape and constrain our perceptual processes, the way in which we experience the world. As we have said before, our experience and understanding of the world motivates our conceptual categories, the meaning of words. Therefore, as we shall see in Chapter 6, our perceptual system must also constrain and affect the way we questioned throughout Western history by various philosophers (see Vinge 1975) and, more recently, by sensory scientists (see Wolfe 1988:xi-xii). B. Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano Polysemy and metaphor in perception verbs 133 conceptualise and use our sense-related language. This is also constrained by the way we – as human beings – perceive perception, that is to say how we think and how we experience the act of perception itself. A full detailed description of human perceptual systems lies outside the scope of this thesis 116 . In the following subsections, however, I will include a brief description of the physiology of each sense, together with a brief discussion of our ‘perception of perception’. These will be extended in Section 5.2, when the properties defining these senses are introduced. As shown in the analysis, these two constraints – sense physiology and our perception – do not always coincide. The way we think we perceive with these senses sometimes does not correspond to the way in which the physiological processes take place. 5.1.1.1. Vision The eyes are the first element in the visual system. Eyes capture light – stimulus for vision – and generate messages about it. The human eye consists of three concentric layers: the fibrous tunic (outermost layer), the vascular tunic (middle layer), and the retina. The function of the outermost layer, the fibrous tunic, is to protect the eyeball. It consists of the sclera (white part) and the cornea (transparent). The function of the middle layer is to nourish the eyeball. It consists of the choroid, a dark pigment that nourishes the retina, and the ciliary body, the structure that contains the aqueous humor that fills the anterior chamber. The ciliary body gives rise to the iris, a circular patch of tissue that gives the eye its characteristic colour. In the middle of the iris lies the pupil, an opening within two sets of muscles. These muscles allow the change of the size of the pupil, which in turn controls the amount of light reaching the back of the eye. Behind the iris, there is an optical element of the eye, the crystalline lens. The lens must be transparent to provide a good vision; an opacity or reduced transparency of the lens – known as cataracts – would cause a deficient visual perception or even blindness. 115 See Howes (1991) for a collection of papers devoted to the anthropology of the senses in different cultures. Ackerman (1990) is also an exploration of the origin and evolution of the senses, as well as their variation across cultures. 116 For a full description of the perceptual systems and processes, see Gibson (1966), Harper (1972), Held (1988), Maelicke (1990), Sekuler and Blake (1994), Wolfe (1988), among others. B. Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano Polysemy and metaphor in perception verbs 134 The lens is composed of three components: the elastic capsule, the epithelial layers and the lens itself. The main function of the capsule is to mould the shape of the lens, thus focusing sharply near or distant objects upon the retina. This process is called ‘accommodation’ (Harper 1972: 115; Sekuler and Blake 1993: 38). Finally, between the lens and the next layer, the retina, there is the vitreous chamber. The retina is the neural tissue at the back of the eye. Some landmarks can be identified in the retina: the macula, in whose centre lies the fovea, the part stimulated by an object in the direct line of sight; the optic disks, where nerve fibres exit the retina carrying information to the brain; and the pigment epithelium. As we have said before, the physical stimulus for vision is light. The visual system can respond to a very large range of stimulus intensities. Light brings information about the objects in the environment (Gibson 1966). However, in order for light to provide information, there are some requirements to be fulfilled (Sekuler and Blake 1994: 44): (i) Light must be sufficiently intense to penetrate the eyes, reaching the photosensitive material in the retina. (ii) The distribution of light must be properly focused. (iii) The pattern of light falling on the retina must preserve the spatial structure of the object from which it is reflected; otherwise, it will not be useful as a source of information about the structure and layout of objects. The incoming light must pass through a complex of neural elements before reaching the photoreceptors in the macula, which are actually responsible for converting light into neural signals. There are two types of photoreceptors: cones, which work in daylight; and rods, which work in dim illumination. These photoreceptors transform the so-called retinal image – the pattern of light distribution reaching the retina – into a neural image. This neural image passes onto a network of diverse cells called collector cells. These cells integrate all the information from groups of neighbouring photoreceptors. The output from the network of collector cells provides the input to the retinal ganglion cells. Due to the centre / surround organisation of their receptive fields, their main task is to detect differences in light level or contrast. These differences, as discussed below, are reflected in language. The axons of the ganglion cells form the optic nerve that provides all the input data for the neural processing of visual information within the brain. The optic nerves B. Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano Polysemy and metaphor in perception verbs 135 from both eyes converge at the optic chiasm. The optic nerve from each eye branches into two segments – one crossed, where fibres cross to the opposite side of the brain; the other uncrossed, where fibres do not change sides. Within the chiasm, crossed fibres from one eye join with uncrossed fibres from the other eye. These new combinations are called optic tracts. Most of these fibres are projected to a cluster of cell bodies called the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN); and the remaining to areas of the midbrain, such as the superior colliculus. This is designed to guide orienting movements of the eyes and head towards detected objects. This ability to orientate the eyes towards the object is reflected in the language in expressions such as point of view and will be represented later in the analysis under the property objects located away from the point of fixation (Sparks 1988). That is why vision, together with hearing, are called the far distance senses: There is no need for the object perceived to be close to the eye 117 . The LGN has two distinct populations of neurones – magnocellular and parvocellular cells. The output from these cells is sent to the visual cortex in the occipital lobe, the major visual centre. The visual cortex is composed of cortical cells that respond to stimulation of a restricted area of the retina. These cells register information about orientation, direction of motion, binocularity and colour. In sum, vision provides us with information about the shape, size, orientation, colour, distance and motion of the OP. In Western society, vision is considered as the most reliable sense, as the sense that offers the most accurate information about the world outside. It is important to notice how the physiology of perception and our perception of perception is sometimes different. As noticed before, vision is believed to be a distant sense. That is to say, we do not need to have contact with what we see. Therefore, despite the fact that human beings perceive vision as an external sense the physical stimulus for vision – light – must penetrate the eyes to be transformed into neural elements. In fact, during the Enlightenment, philosophers saw in this detachment from the eyes and the OP the basis for the ‘objectivity’ of vision, and hence, the basis for the scientific value of this sense. Properties > and > in Section 5.2.2 reflect this fact. 117 This is represented by the property > in Section 5.2.2. B. Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano Polysemy and metaphor in perception verbs 136 Analogies between the physiology of vision and metaphorical expressions in language are very obvious. For instance, the fact that the eyes are the most important element in the visual system is reflected in expressions like I couldn’t believe it until I Download 1.39 Mb. Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |
Ma'lumotlar bazasi mualliflik huquqi bilan himoyalangan ©fayllar.org 2024
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling
ma'muriyatiga murojaat qiling