also includes
the knowledge of
vocabulary, phonology,
and
graphology
under the general heading of
grammatical knowledge. Both
grammatical knowledge
(grammar at the sentential and sub•
sentential levels) and
te.xtual knowledge
(language at the discourse level) are subsumed
under
organizational knowledge
(how language and structures are produced to form gram
matically correct utterances and sentences).
Bachman and Palmer's work also built on earlier notions of
communicative competence
(cf. Hymes, 1974; Canale and Swain, 1980), which refers to a language user's ability to
use his
I
her
grammatical knowledge
(e.g., syntax, morphology) to communicate through
spoken utterances and writing. In this way, grammar is not only a set of abstract rules, but is
fundamental to the creation of meaning. Thus, for Purpura (2004 ),
grammatical kllowledgc
embodies two closely related components:
grammatical form
at the subsentential, sentence,
and discourse levels, and
grammatical meaning,
which includes both the literal and intended
meaning of utterances and sentences as a way of accounting for "meaning in both content
impoverished (e.g., multiple-choice tasks) and context-rich (e.g., problem-solving tasks)
test situations" (p. 63).
Finally, for testing purposes, an important distinction needs to be made between
knowledge
and
ability.
As they relate to grammar, a person's
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |